Who is a thinker? With age, higher SES American children increasingly associate social status with divisions in labour
Corresponding Author
Shaylene E. Nancekivell
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada
Correspondence
Shaylene E. Nancekivell, Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada.
Email: [email protected]
Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing
Search for more papers by this authorTatyana Farrow
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Contribution: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorBrian A. Maurer
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Contribution: Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Shaylene E. Nancekivell
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada
Correspondence
Shaylene E. Nancekivell, Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada.
Email: [email protected]
Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing
Search for more papers by this authorTatyana Farrow
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Contribution: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorBrian A. Maurer
Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
Contribution: Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing - original draft
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This exploratory report investigates how children, aged 6- to 12-years, reason about divisions in labour. It focuses on understanding when in development children might associate higher status groups with intellectual as opposed to physical labour. It explores this question by introducing a sample of mostly mid/high-SES American children to a novel factory setting and then asking them who is likely to have one of two jobs: a ‘builder’ (physical labour), or ‘thinker’ (intellectual labour) job. Older children were more likely than younger children to associate an individual's higher social status with intellectual labour work as opposed to physical labour work. Children also explained their reasoning, and with age their explanations focused more on social factors like the role of access to ‘choices’ or opportunities in shaping the nature of others' work.
Open Research
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The majority of the data that support the findings of this study are openly available in OSF at https://osf.io/5d9qy/?view_only=83c13d06da11442fb39e2e88106c7da1.
REFERENCES
- Ahl, R. E., & Dunham, Y. (2019). “Wealth makes many friends”: Children expect more giving from resource-rich than resource-poor individuals. Child Development, 90(2), 524–543. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12922
- Bian, L., Leslie, S. J., & Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children's interests. Science, 355(6323), 389–391.
- Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 68(3), 530–543. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01956.x
- Chafel, J. A., & Neitzel, C. (2005). Young children's ideas about the nature, causes, justification, and alleviation of poverty. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(4), 433–450.
- Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communications in Statistics: Simulation and Computation, 39(4), 860–864.
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82(3), 793–811. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
- Elenbaas, L. (2019). Against unfairness: Young children's judgments about merit, equity, and equality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 73–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.009
- Elenbaas, L., & Mistry, R. S. (2021). Distributive justice in society and among peers: 8-to 14-year-olds' views on economic stratification inform their decisions about access to opportunities. Developmental Psychology, 57(6), 951–961.
- Elenbaas, L., Rizzo, M. T., Cooley, S., & Killen, M. (2016). Rectifying social inequalities in a resource allocation task. Cognition, 155, 176–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.002
- Elenbaas, L., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2020). A developmental-science perspective on social inequality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(6), 610–616.
- Enright, E. A., Alonso, D. J., Lee, B. M., & Olson, K. R. (2020). Children's understanding and use of four dimensions of social status. Journal of Cognition and Development, 21(4), 573–602. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2020.1797745
- Flanagan, C. A., Kim, T., Pykett, A., Finlay, A., Gallay, E. E., & Pancer, M. (2014). Adolescents' theories about economic inequality: Why are some people poor while others are rich? Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2512–2525. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037934
- Gülgöz, S., & Gelman, S. A. (2017). Who's the boss? Concepts of social power across development. Child Development, 88(3), 946–963.
- Jaxon, J., Lei, R. F., Shachnai, R., Chestnut, E. K., & Cimpian, A. (2019). The acquisition of gender stereotypes about intellectual ability: Intersections with race. Journal of Social Issues, 75(4), 1192–1215.
- Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2001). Pink and blue collar jobs: Children's judgments of job status and job aspirations in relation to sex of worker. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(4), 346–363. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2611
- Mandalaywala, T. M., Tai, C., & Rhodes, M. (2020). Children's use of race and gender as cues to social status. PLoS One, 15(6), e0234398.
- McLoyd, V. C. (2019). How children and adolescents think about, make sense of, and respond to economic inequality: Why does it matter? Developmental Psychology, 55(3), 592–600. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000691
- Olson, K. R., Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., & Weisman, K. G. (2012). Children associate racial groups with wealth: Evidence from South Africa. Child Development, 83(6), 1884–1899. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01819.x
- Paulus, M. (2014). The early origins of human charity: Developmental changes in preschoolers' sharing with poor and wealthy individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 344. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00344
- Peretz-Lange, R., Perry, J., & Muentener, P. (2021). Developmental shifts toward structural explanations and interventions for social status disparities. Cognitive Development, 58, 101042. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101042
10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101042 Google Scholar
- Powell, W. W., & Snellman, K. (2004). The knowledge economy. Annual Review of Sociology, 30, 199–220. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.29.010202.100037
- Ramsey, P. G. (1991). Young children's awareness and understanding of social class differences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 152(1), 71–82.
- Roberts, S. O., Ho, A. K., Gülgöz, S., Berka, J., & Gelman, S. A. (2020). The roles of group status and group membership in the practice of hypodescent. Child Development, 91(3), e721–e732. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13279
- Scheresky, R. F. (1977). Occupational roles are sex-typed by six-to ten-year-old children. Psychology in the Schools, 14(2), 220–224.
- Shutts, K., Brey, E. L., Dornbusch, L. A., Slywotzky, N., & Olson, K. R. (2016). Children use wealth cues to evaluate others. PLoS One, 11(3), e0149360. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149360
- Sigelman, C. K. (2012). Rich man, poor man: Developmental differences in attributions and perceptions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(3), 415–429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.011
- Taylor, S. I., Wang, L. W., VanBrackle, A., & Kaneda, T. (2003). What I want to be when I grow up: A qualitative study of American and Japanese children's occupational aspirations. Child Study Journal, 33(3), 175–187.
- Teig, S., & Susskind, J. E. (2008). Truck driver or nurse? The impact of gender roles and occupational status on children's occupational preferences. Sex Roles, 58(11–12), 848–863. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9410-x
- Woods, T. A., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Rowley, S. J. (2005). The development of stereotypes about the rich and poor: Age, race, and family income differences in beliefs. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(5), 437–445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-7261-0
- World Bank. (2019). World development report 2019: The changing nature of work. World Bank Group. https://doi.org/10.1596/978-1-4648-1328-3
10.1596/978-1-4648-1328-3 Google Scholar