Are Clinicians Better Than Lay Judges at Recalling Case Details? An Evaluation of Expert Memory
Corresponding Author
Christopher A. Webb
Mississippi State University
Please address correspondence to: Christopher A. Webb, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 6161, Mississippi State University, Mississippi State, MS 39762. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Christopher A. Webb
Mississippi State University
Please address correspondence to: Christopher A. Webb, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 6161, Mississippi State University, Mississippi State, MS 39762. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorWe thank the many undergraduate research assistants who helped prepare recruitment letters and code data. This project would not have been possible without them.
Abstract
Objective
This study examined the role of expertise in clinicians’ memory for case details. Clinicians’ diagnostic formulations may afford mechanisms for retaining and retrieving information.
Method
Experts (N = 41; 47.6% males, 23.8% females; 28.6% did not report gender; age: mean [M] = 54.69) were members of the American Board of Professional Psychologists. Lay judges (N = 156; 25.4% males, 74.1% females; age: M = 18.85) were undergraduates enrolled in general psychology. Three vignettes were presented to each group, creating a 2 (group: expert, lay judge) x 3 (vignettes: simple, complex–coherent, complex–incoherent) mixed factorial design. Recall accuracy for vignette details was the dependent variable.
Results
Data analyses used multivariate analyses of variance to detect group differences among multiple continuous variables. Experts recalled more information than lay judges, overall. However, experts also exhibited more false memories for the complex–incoherent case because of their schema-based knowledge.
Conclusions
This study supported clinical expertise as beneficial. Nonetheless, negative influences from experts’ schema-based knowledge, as exhibited, could adversely affect clinical practices.
References
- Aegisdottir, S., White, M. J., Spengler, P. M., Maugherman, A. S., Anderson, L. A., Cook, R. S., … Rush, J. D. (2006). The meta-analysis of clinical judgment project: Fifty-six years of accumulated research on clinical versus statistical prediction. The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 341–382. doi:10.1177/0011000005285875
- Ahn, W., Flanagan, E. H., Marsh, J. K., & Sanislow, C. A. (2006). Beliefs about essences and the reality of mental disorders. Psychological Science, 17(9), 759–766. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01779.x
- American Board of Professional Psychologists. (n.d.). Application steps and generic requirements. Retrieved from http://www.abpp.org/i4a/pages/index.cfm?pageid = 3297
- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author.
- American Psychological Association. (n.d.). Frequently asked questions about graduate school. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/education/grad/faqs.aspx
- American Psychological Association. (2012). Division 39 Practice survey: A guide to the main results. Retrieved from http://www.apadivisions.org/division-39/publications/review/2012/07/practice-survey-guide.aspx
- Anderson, R. C. (1984). Role of the reader's schema in comprehension, learning, and memory. In R. C. Anderson, J. Osborn, & R. Tierney (Eds.), Learning to read in American schools: Basal readers and content texts (pp. 243–257). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Anderson, J. R. (2005). Cognitive psychology and its implications ( 6th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.
- Baird, R. R. (2001). Experts sometimes show more false recall than novices: A cost of knowing too much. Learning and Individual Differences, 13(4), 349–355. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(03)00018-9
- Bedard, J., & Chi, M. T. (1992). Expertise. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 1(4), 135–139. doi.10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769799
10.1111/1467‐8721.ep10769799 Google Scholar
- Behrmann, M., & Ewell, C. (2003). Expertise in tactile pattern recognition. Psychological Science, 14(5), 480–486. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02458
- Berry, J., Abernethy, B., & Cote, J. (2008). The contribution of structured activity and deliberate play to the development of expert perceptual and decision-making skill. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30(6), 685–708.
- Brewer, W. F., & Nakamura, G. V. (1984). The nature and functions of schemas. In R. R. Wyer, T. K. Srull, R. R. Wyer, & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol 1, pp. 118–160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
- Borsboom, D., Cramer, A., Schmittmann, V. D., Epskamp, S., & Waldorp, L. J. (2011). The small world of psychopathology. PLoS ONE, 6(11), e27407. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027407
- Chi, M. H. (2006a). Laboratory methods for assessing experts’ and novices’ knowledge. In K. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, R. R. Hoffman, K. Ericsson, N. Charness, & R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 167–184). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Chi, M. H. (2006b). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In K. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, R. R. Hoffman, K. Ericsson, N. Charness, & R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21–30). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Choudhry, N. K., Fletcher, R. H., & Soumerai, S. B. (2005). Systematic review: The relationship between clinical experience and quality of health care. Annuals of Internal Medicine, 142(4), 260–273.
- Cohen, G. (2008). Memory for knowledge: General knowledge and expert knowledge. In G. Cohen, M. A. Conway, G. Cohen, M. A. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world ( 3rd ed., pp. 207–227). New York: Psychology Press.
- Connors, M. H., Burns, B. D., & Campitelli, G. (2011). Expertise in complex decision making: The role of search in chess 70 years after de Groot. Cognitive Science, 35, 1567–1579. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01196.x
- Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(4), 594–628. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005
- Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychiatry built on myth. New York: Free Press.
- Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.
- de Groot, A. D. (1964). Thought and choice in chess ( 1st ed.). Oxford, UK: Mouton.
- Duff, K., Callister, C., Dennett, K., & Toetich, D. (2012). Practice effects: A unique cognitive variable. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(7), 1117–1127. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.722685
- Ericsson, K. (2009). Enhancing the development of professional performance: Implication from the study of deliberate practice. In K. Ericsson, K. Ericsson, (Eds.), Development of professional expertise: Toward measurement of expert performance and design of optimal learning environments (pp. 405–431). New York: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511609817.022 Google Scholar
- Ericsson, K., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49(8), 725–747. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.725
- Ericsson, K., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 10(2), 211–245. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211
- Ericsson, K., & Pennington, N. (1993). The structure of memory performance in experts: Implications for memory in everyday life. In G. M. Davies & R. H. Logie (Eds.), Memory in everyday life: Advances in psychology (Vol. 100, pp. 241–277). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.
10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61105-X Google Scholar
- Ericsson, K., Roring, R. W., & Nandagopal, K. (2007). Giftedness and evidence for reproducibly superior performance: An account based on the expert performance framework. High Ability Studies, 18(1), 3–56. doi:10.1080/13598130701350593
- Ericsson, K., & Ward, P. (2007). Capturing the naturally occurring superior performance of experts in the laboratory: Toward a science of expert and exceptional performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 346–350. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00533.x
- Flanagan, E., Keeley, J., & Blashfield, R. K. (2012). Do clinicians’ taxonomies of mental disorders match the hierarchical structure of the DSM? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 620–630.
- Fouad, N. A., Grus, C. L., Hatcher, R. L., Kaslow, N. J., Hutchings, P. S., Madson, M. B., … Crossman, R. E. (2009). Competency benchmarks: A model for understanding and measuring competence in professional psychology across training levels. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 3, S5–S26.
- Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 833–848.
- Garb, H. N. (2005). Clinical judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 67–89. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143810
- Grove, W., Zald, D., Lebow, B., Snitz, B., & Nelson, C. (2000). Clinical versus mechanical prediction: A meta-analysis. Psychological Assessment, 12, 19–30.
- Hunt, R., & Rawson, K. A. (2011). Knowledge affords distinctive processing in memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 390–405. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.001
- Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002). Clinical psychologists’ theory-based representations of mental disorders predict their diagnostic reasoning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(4), 451–476.
- Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: The contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 5–11. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.5
- McCaffrey, R. J., Duff, K., & Westervelt, H. (2000). Practitioner's guide to evaluating change with neuropsychological assessment instruments. Dordrecht Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
10.1007/978-1-4615-4233-9 Google Scholar
- Medin, D. L., Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome? Cognitive Psychology, 32(1), 49–96. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0645
- Meehl, P. (1954). Clinical vs. statistical prediction: A theoretical analysis and a review of the evidence. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
- Mehta, R., Hoegg, J., & Chakravarti, A. (2011). Knowing too much: Expertise-induced false recall effects in product comparison. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(3), 535–554. doi:10.1086/659380
- Moore, C. D., Cohen, M. X., & Ranganath, C. (2006). Neural mechanisms of expert skills in visual working memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(43), 11187–11196. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1873-06.2006
- Neath, I. (1998). Human memory: An introduction to research, data, and theory. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.
- Norman, G. R., Brooks, L. R., & Allen, S. W. (1989). Recall by expert medical practitioners and novices as a record of processing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 1166–1174.
- Patel, V. L., Arocha, J. F., & Kauffman, D. R. (1994). Diagnostic reasoning and medical expertise. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 187–251). New York: Academic
- Patel, V. L., & Groen, G. L. (1991). The general and specific nature of medical expertise: A critical look. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise (pp. 93–125). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press
- Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 803–814.
- Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & Robinson, K. J. (1998). The role of associative processes in producing false remembering. In M. A. Conway, S. Gathercole, & C. Cornoldi (Eds.), Theories of memory II (pp. 187–245). Hove, Sussex: Psychological Press.
- Rouder, J. N., & Ratcliff, R. (2006). Comparing exemplar- and rule-based theories of categorization. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 15(1), 9–13. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00397.x
- Shanteau, J. (1992). Competence in experts: The role of task characteristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53, 256–266. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(92)90064-E
- Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. (2005). Distinctive processing improves memory for recent autobiographical events. In D. T. Rosen (Ed.), Trends in experimental psychology research (pp. 269–281). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
- Strohmer, D. C., Shivy, V. A., & Chiodo, A. L. (1990). Information processing strategies in counselor hypothesis testing: The role of selective memory and expectancy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37(4), 465–472.
- Tracey, T., Wampold, B., Lichtenberg, J., & Goodyear, R. (2014). Expertise in psychotherapy. An elusive goal? American Psychologist, 69(3), 218–229. doi:10.1037/a0035099
- Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. J., Van Breukelen, G. P., & Jolles, J. (2007). Assessment of information processing in working memory in applied settings: The paper & pencil memory scanning test. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research in Psychiatry and the Allied Sciences, 37(9), 1335–1344. doi:10.1017/S0033291707000360
- Witteman, C., & Tollenaar, M. (2012). Remembering and diagnosing clients: Does experience matter? Memory, 20(3), 266–276. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.654799
- Witteman, C., & Van den Bercken, J. (2007). Intermediate effects in psychodianostic classification. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(1), 56–61. doi:10.1027/1015-5759
- Witteman, C., Weiss, D., & Metzmacher, M. (2012). Assessing diagnostic expertise of counselors using the Cochran-Weiss-Shanteau (CWS) Index. Journal of Counseling and Development, 90, 30–34. doi:10.1111/j.1556-6676.2012.00005.x