Multitasking
Abstract
Multitasking has become increasingly prevalent, especially as we continue to incorporate more and more new media technologies into our daily activities. This essay first identifies trends in the availability and use of media devices in daily life and multitasking behaviors related to such trends. Second, given the general consensus that multitasking impairs performance outcomes, recent multitasking trends call for greater research attention to the subject. We outline the historical perspectives on cognitive structures and processes related to a human's general ability to multitask, culminating with the more recent threaded cognition theory. Third, we present two new research directions on multitasking. One is the exploration of long-term consequences of multitasking behaviors, such as their impacts on cognitive functions, and dynamic changes in individuals' needs and multitasking behavioral changes over time; the other is a cognitive dimensional framework for defining multitasking, which may offer a means to reconcile findings across various multitasking research paradigms, and also to guide designs of multitasking technologies and environments. Finally, looking to the future, we propose several ways to advance the research on multitasking.
References
- Adler, R. F., & Benbunan-Fich, R. (2012). Juggling on a high wire: Multitasking effects on performance. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 70, 156–168.
- Allport, D. A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225–235.
-
Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013, February 11). The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0031208
10.1037/a0031208 Google Scholar
- Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.
- Armstrong, G. B., & Chung, L. (2000). Background television and reading memory in context. Communication Research, 27, 327–352.
- Basil, M. D. (1994). Multiple resource theory I: Application to television viewing. Communication Research, 21, 177–207.
- Bauerlein, M. (2008). The dumbest generation: How the digital age stupefies young Americans and jeopardizes our future. New York, NY: Jeremy P. Tarcher/Penguin Group.
- Bowman, L. L., Levine, L. E., Waite, B. M., & Gendron, M. (2010). Can students really multitask? An experimental study of instant messaging while reading. Computers & Education, 54, 927–931.
- Cantril, H., & Allport, G. W. (1935). The psychology of radio (pp. 19–35). New York, NY: Harper & Brothers.
- Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Benitez, S., & Chang, J. (2009). Multitasking across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 483–489.
- Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 467–490.
- Dux, P. E., Ivanoff, J. G., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron, 52, 1109–1120.
-
Hamilton, J. (1859). Lectures on metaphysics and logic. Edinburgh, UK: Blackwood.
10.1037/12154-000 Google Scholar
- Havender, J. W. (1968). Avoiding deadlock in multitasking systems. IBM Systems Journal, 7, 74–84.
- Hawkins, R. P., Pingree, S., Hitchon, J., Radler, B., Gorham, B. W., Kahlor, L., & Kolbeins, G. H. (2005). What produces television attention and attention style? Human Communication Research, 31, 162–187.
- Jackson, M. (2008). Distracted: The erosion of attention and the coming dark age. New York, NY: Prometheus Books.
- Jeong, S. H., & Fishbein, M. (2007). Predictors of multitasking with media: Media factors and audience factors. Journal of Media Psychology, 10, 364–384.
- Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
- Kirn, W. (2007) (November). The autumn of the multitaskers. The Atlantic. Retrieved from http://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/toc/2007/11.
- La Cerra, P., & Bingham, R. (1998). The adaptive nature of the human neurocognitive architecture: An alternative model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 11290–11294.
- Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication, 50, 46–70.
- Lang, A. (2006). Using the limited capacity model of motivated mediated message processing to design effective cancer communication messages. Journal of Communication, 56, S57–S80.
- Levy, J., & Pashler, H. (2008). Task prioritization in multitasking during driving: Opportunity to abort a concurrent task does not insulate braking responses from dual-task slowing. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 507–525.
- Levy, J., Pashler, H., & Boer, E. (2006). Central interference in driving: Is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Science, 17, 228–235.
-
Lin, L., Robertson, T., & Lee, J. (2009). Reading performances between novices and experts in different media multitasking environments. Computers in the Schools, 26, 169–186.
10.1080/07380560903095162 Google Scholar
- Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 296–305.
- Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2002). Animation as an aid to multimedia learning. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 87–99.
- McCartt, A. T., Hellinga, L. A., & Bratiman, K. A. (2006). Cell phones and driving: Review of research. Traffic Injury Prevention, 7(2), 89–106.
- Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
- Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358–368.
- Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.
- Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15583–15587.
- Oswald, F. L., Hambrick, D. Z., & Jones, L. A. (2007). Keeping all the plates spinning: Understanding and predicting multitasking performance. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Learning to solve complex scientific problems (pp. 77–97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation M 2 : Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. Retrieved from the Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation: http://kff.org/other/event/generation-m2-media-in-the-lives-of/.
- Salvucci, D. D., & Taatgen, N. A. (2008). Threaded cognition: An integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychological Review, 115, 101–130.
- Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Strayer, D. L., Medeiros-Ward, N., & Watson, J. M. (2013). Who multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. PLoS ONE, 8, e54402.
-
Svoen, B. (2007). Consumers, participants, and creators: Young people's diverse use of television and new media. ACM Computers in Entertainment, 5(2), 1–16.
10.1145/1279540.1279545 Google Scholar
- Townsend, J. T. (1990). Serial vs. parallel processing: Sometimes they look like tweedledum and tweedledee but they can (and should) be distinguished. Psychological Science, 1, 46–54.
- Townsend, J. T., & Wenger, M. J. (2004). A theory of interactive parallel processing: New capacity measures and predictions for a response time inequality series. Psychological Review, 111, 1003–1035.
- Wang, Z., David, P., Srivastava, J., Powers, S., Brady, C., D'Angelo, J., & Moreland, J. (2012). Behavioral performance and visual attention in communication multitasking: A comparison between instant messaging and online voice chat. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 968–975.
-
Wang, Z., Irwin, M., Cooper, C., & Srivastava, J. (2014). Multidimensions of media multitasking and adaptive media selection. Human Communication Research, Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/hcre.12042
10.1111/hcre.12042 Google Scholar
- Wang, Z., Lang, A., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2011). Motivational processing and choice behavior during television viewing: An integrative dynamic approach. Journal of Communication, 61, 71–93.
- Wang, Z., & Tchernev, J. M. (2012). The “myth” of media multitasking: Reciprocal dynamics of media multitasking, personal needs, and gratifications. Journal of Communication, 62, 493–513.
- Welford, A. T. (1967). Single-channel operation in the brain. Acta Psychologica, 27, 5–22.
-
Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159–177.
10.1080/14639220210123806 Google Scholar
- Zhang, W., & Zhang, L. (2012). Explicating multitasking with computers: Gratifications and situations. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1883–1891.
Further Reading
- Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.
- Moreno, M. A., Jelenchick, L., Koff, R., Eikoff, J., Diermyer, C., & Christakis, D. A. (2012). Internet use and multitasking among older adolescents: An experience sampling approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1097–1102.
- Nielsen Company (2012). State of the media: Cross-platform report quarter 2, 2012—US. Available from the Nielsen Company, http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/reports/2012/state-of-the-media–cross-platform-report-q2-2012.html.
- Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.
- Pool, M. M., Koolstra, C. M., & van der Voort, T. (2003). The impact of background radio and television on high school students' homework performance. Journal of Communication, 53, 74–87.
- Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: Uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.
Citing Literature
Browse other articles of this reference work: