Stability and reliability of error-related electromyography over the corrugator supercilii with increasing trials
Corresponding Author
Nathaniel Elkins-Brown
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Correspondence Nathaniel Elkins-Brown, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 1265 Military Trail, Toronto, ON M1C 1A4, Canada. Email: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorBlair Saunders
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorFrank He
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorMichael Inzlicht
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Nathaniel Elkins-Brown
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Correspondence Nathaniel Elkins-Brown, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 1265 Military Trail, Toronto, ON M1C 1A4, Canada. Email: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorBlair Saunders
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorFrank He
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorMichael Inzlicht
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorFunding information: Canada's Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) and Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) grants (to M. I.)
Abstract
Electromyographic activity over the corrugator supercilii (cEMG), the primary facial muscle involved in negative emotions, is increased during the commission of errors on speeded reaction-time tasks. In the present paper, data from two previously published studies were reanalyzed to investigate the reliability and stability of error-related, correct-related, and difference cEMG across increasing numbers of trials. For a modified go/no-go and a flanker task, we found that error-related cEMG was highly stable and reliable in 14 trials, and correct-related cEMG between 56 and 82 trials, respectively. Given the typical number of trials used in studies of cognitive control, these findings suggest that many investigations of error monitoring are already sufficient to obtain acceptable error- and correct-related cEMG signals. Error-related cEMG activity is relatively easy to measure and, as such, it shows great promise for future research investigating the cognitive and affective mechanisms of error monitoring.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.
Filename | Description |
---|---|
psyp12902-sup-0001-suppinfo01.docx499.7 KB |
Figure S1 Figure S2 Figure S3 Figure S4 Figure S5 Figure S6 |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
REFERENCES
- Aarts, K., De Houwer, J., & Pourtois, G. (2013). Erroneous and correct actions have a difference affective valence: Evidence from ERPs. Emotion, 13(5), 960–973. doi:10.1037/a0032808
- Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as principled argument. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 356–366. doi:10.3758/cabn.7.4.356
- Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 260–268. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.260
- J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511546396 Google Scholar
- Chetverikov, A., & Kristjánsson, Á (2016). On the joys of perceiving: Affect as feedback for perceptual predictions. Acta Psychologica, 169, 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.05.005
- Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2007). Repetition and event-related potentials: Distinguishing early and late processes in affective picture perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(4), 577–586. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.577
- Codispoti, M., Mazzetti, M., & Bradley, M. M. (2009). Unmasking emotion: Exposure duration and emotional engagement. Psychophysiology, 46(4), 731–738. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00804.x
- Cohen, J. (1973). Eta-squared and partial eta-squared in fixed factor ANOVA designs. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 33, 107–112. doi:10.1177/001316447303300111
- Cohen, J., & Polich, J. (1997). On the number of trials needed for P300. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 25(3), 249–255. doi:10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00743-x
- Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain (pp. 140–142). New York, NY: Putnam,
- de Morree, H. M., & Marcora, S. M. (2010). The face of effort; frowning muscle activity reflects effort during a physical task. Biological Psychology, 85(3), 377–382. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.08.009
- de Morree, H. M., & Marcora, S. M. (2012). Frowning muscle activity and perception of effort during constant-workload cycling. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 112, 1967–1972. doi:10.1007/s00421-011-2138-2
- DeSteno, D., Li, Y., Dickens, L., & Lerner, J. (2014). Gratitude: A tool for reducing economic impatience. Psychological Science, 25, 1262–1267. doi:10.1177/0956797614529979.
- de Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Zaalberg, R., Goudena, P. P., & Matthys, W. (2006). Facial EMG responses to dynamic emotional facial expressions in boys with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40(2), 112–121. doi:101016/j.jpsychires.2005.08.003
- Edwards, L. J., Muller, K. E., Wolfinger, R. D., Qaqish, B. F., & Schabenberger, O. (2008). An R2 statistic for fixed effects in the linear mixed model. Statistics in Medicine, 27, 6137–6157. doi:10.1002/sim.3429
- Elkins-Brown, N., Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2016). Error-related electromyographic activity over the corrugator supercilii is associated with neural performance monitoring. Psychophysiology, 53(2), 159–170. doi:10.1111/psyp.12556
- Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi:10.3758/bf03203267.
- Foti, D., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Psychometric considerations in using error-related brain activity as a biomarker in psychotic disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(2), 520–531. doi:10.1037/a0032618
- Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385–390. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x
-
Gerdle, B.,
Karlsson, S.,
Day, S., &
Djupsjöbacka, M. (1999). Acquisition, processing and analysis of the surface electromyogram. In U. Windhorst & H. Johansson (Eds.), Modern techniques in neuroscience research (pp. 705–755). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
10.1007/978-3-642-58552-4_26 Google Scholar
- Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2011). Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: A critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology, 48(12), 1711–1725. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x
-
Halaki, M., &
Ginn, K. (2012). Normalization of EMG signals: To normalize or not to normalize and what to normalize to? In G. R. Naik (Ed.), Computational intelligence in electromyography analysis—A perspective on current applications and future challenges. Rijeka, Croatia: Intech. doi:10.5772/49957
10.5772/49957 Google Scholar
-
Hinton, P. R.,
Brownlow, C.,
McMurray, I., &
Cozens, B. (2004). SPSS explained. East Sussex, UK: Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781315797298
10.4324/9781315797298 Google Scholar
- Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679–709. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.679
- Inzlicht, M., Bartholow, B. D., & Hirsh, J. B. (2015). Emotional foundations of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 126–132. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.004
- Janis, J. E., Ghavami, A., Lemmon, J. A., Leedy, J. E., & Guyuron, B. (2007). Anatomy of the corrugator supercilii muscle: Part I. Corrugator topography. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 120(6), 1647–1653. doi:10.1097/01.prs.0000282725.61640.e1
- Kaye, J. T., Bradford, D. E., & Curtin, J. J. (2016). Psychometric properties of startle and corrugator response in NPU, affective picture viewing, and resting state tasks. Psychophysiology, 53(8), 1241–1255. doi:10.1111/psyp. 12663
- Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. doi:10.1037/a0020198
- Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x
- Larson, M. J., Baldwin, S. A., Good, D. A., & Fair, J. E. (2010). Temporal stability of the error-related negativity (ERN) and post-error positivity (Pe): The role of number of trials. Psychophysiology, 47(6), 1167–1171. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01022.x
- Larsen, J. T., Norris, C. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40(5), 776–785. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00078
- Lindström, B. R., Mattsson-Mårn, I. B., Golkar, A., & Olsson, A. (2013). In your face: Risk of punishment enhances cognitive control and error-related activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle. PLOS ONE, 8(6), e65692.
- Marco-Pallares, J., Cucurell, D., Münte, T. F., Strien, N., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2011). On the number of trials needed for a stable feedback-related negativity. Psychophysiology, 48(6), 852–860. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01152.x
- Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024
- Mathiassen, S. E., Winkel, J., & Hägg, G. M. (1995). Normalization of surface EMG amplitude from the upper trapezius muscle in ergonomic studies—A review. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 5(4), 197–226. doi:10.1016/1050-6411(94)00014-x
- Matzke, B., Herpertz, S. C., Berger, C., Fleischer, M., & Domes, G. (2013). Facial reactions during emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: A facial electromyography study. Psychopathology, 47(2), 101–110. doi:10.1159/000351122
- Meyer, D. E., Riesel, A., & Proudfit, G. H. (2013). Reliability of ERN across multiple tasks as a function of increasing errors. Psychophysiology, 50, 1220–1225. doi:10.1111/psyp.12132
- Moran, T. P., Jendrusina, A. A., & Moser, J. S. (2013). The psychometric properties of the late positive potential during emotion processing and regulation. Brain Research, 1516, 66–75. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.04.018
- Morecraft, R. J., McNeal, D. W., Stillwell-Morecraft, K. S., Gedney, M., Ge, J., Schroeder, C. M., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (2007). Amygdala interconnections with the cingulate motor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 500(1), 134–165. doi:10.1002/cne.21165
- Morecraft, R. J., Stilwell–Morecraft, K. S., & Rossing, W. R. (2004). The motor cortex and facial expression: New insights from neuroscience. Neurologist, 10(5), 235–249. doi:10.1097/01.nrl.0000138734.45742.8d
- Morecraft, R. J., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (1993). Frontal granular cortex input to the cingulate (M3), supplementary (M2) and primary (M1) motor cortices in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 337(4), 669–689. doi:10.1002/cne.903370411
- Morecraft, R. J., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (1998). Convergence of limbic input to the cingulate motor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Brain Research Bulletin, 45(2), 209–232. doi:10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00344-4
- Morecraft, R. J., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of limbic structures and some relationships with prefrontal cortex. In R. B. Schiffer, S. M. Rao, & B. S. Fogel (Eds.), Neuropsychiatry (pp. 294–327). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins.
- Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2009). The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 957–961. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x
- Pontifex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Brown, M. L., O'Leary, K. C., Wu, C. T., Themanson, J. R., & Hillman, C. H. (2010). On the number of trials necessary for stabilization of error-related brain activity across the life span. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 767–773. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00974.x
- Pope, L. K., & Smith, C. A. (1994). On the distinct meanings of smiles and frowns. Cognition & Emotion, 8(1), 65–72. doi:10.1080/02699939408408929
- Saunders, B., He, F., & Inzlicht, M. (2015). Does gratitude improve self-regulation? No evidence that gratefulness enhances neural performance monitoring or conflict-driven control. PLOS ONE, 10(12), e0143312. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143312
- Saunders, B., Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M., (2015). What does cognitive control feel like? Effective and ineffective cognitive control is associated with divergent phenomenology. Psychophysiology, 52, 1205–1217. doi:10.1111/psyp.12454
- Schacht, A., Dimigen, O., & Sommer, W. (2010). Emotions in cognitive conflicts are not aversive but are task specific. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(3), 349–356. doi:10.3758/CABN.10.3.349
- Schacht, A., Nigbur, R., & Sommer, W. (2009). Emotions in go/nogo conflicts. Psychological Research, 73, 843–856. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0192-0
- Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(3), 154–167. doi:10.1038/nrn2994
- Schüpbach, R. L., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2014). Contrasting the effects of suboptimally versus optimally presented affect primes on effort-related cardiac response. Motivation and Emotion, 38(6), 748–758. doi:10.1007/s11031-014-9438-x
- Simmonds, D. J., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2008). Meta-analysis of go/no-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 224–232. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.015
- Smith, D. P., Hillman, C. H., & Duley, A. R. (2005). Influences of age on emotional reactivity during picture processing. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(1), P49–P56. doi:10.1093/geronb/60.1.p49
- Topolinski, S., Erle, T. M., & Reber, R. (2015). Necker's smile: Immediate affective consequences of early perceptual processes. Cognition, 140, 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.004
- Topolinski, S., Likowski, K. U., Weyers, P., & Strack, F. (2009). The face of fluency: Semantic coherence automatically elicits a specific pattern of facial muscle reactions. Cognition & Emotion, 23(2), 260–271. doi:10.1080/02699930801994112
- Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Corrugator activity confirms immediate negative affect in surprise. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00134
- van Boxtel, A. (2010). Facial EMG as a tool for inferring affective states. In Proceedings of Measuring Behavior. Wageningen, Netherlands: Noldus Information Technology.
- van Boxtel, A., & Jessurun, M. (1993). Amplitude and bilateral coherency of facial and jaw-elevator EMG activity as an index of effort during a two-choice serial reaction task. Psychophysiology, 30(6), 589–604. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02085.x
- Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Longer term test–retest reliability of error-related brain activity. Psychophysiology, 48(10), 1420–1425. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01206.x
- Weinberg, A., Riesel, A., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Integrating multiple perspectives on error-related brain activity: The ERN as a neural indicator of trait defensive reactivity. Motivation and Emotion, 36(1), 84–100. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9269-y
- Xu, X., & Inzlicht, M. (2015). Neurophysiological responses to gun-shooting errors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 95(3), 247–253. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.015