Inducing unconscious stress: Cardiovascular activity in response to subliminal presentation of threatening and neutral words
Corresponding Author
Melanie M. van der Ploeg
Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Correspondence Melanie van der Ploeg, Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. Email: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorJos F. Brosschot
Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorBart Verkuil
Clinical Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorBrandon L. Gillie
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorDeWayne P. Williams
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorJulian Koenig
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centre for Psychosocial Medicine, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorMichael W. Vasey
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorJulian F. Thayer
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Melanie M. van der Ploeg
Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Correspondence Melanie van der Ploeg, Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. Email: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorJos F. Brosschot
Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorBart Verkuil
Clinical Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorBrandon L. Gillie
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorDeWayne P. Williams
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorJulian Koenig
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centre for Psychosocial Medicine, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorMichael W. Vasey
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorJulian F. Thayer
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio
Search for more papers by this authorFunding information: ZON-MW (Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development; TOP Grant, nr. 40-00812-98-11029) (to J. F. B.), Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NOW, Veni grant 451-14-013) (to B. V.)
Abstract
Stress-related cognitive processes may occur outside of awareness, here referred to as unconscious stress, and affect one's physiological state. Evidence supporting this idea would provide necessary clarification of the relationship between psychological stress and cardiovascular (CV) health problems. We tested the hypothesis that increases in mean arterial pressure (MAP) and total peripheral resistance (TPR) and decreases in heart rate variability (HRV) would be larger when threatening stimuli are presented outside of awareness, or subliminally, compared with neutral stimuli. Additionally, it was expected that trait worry and resting HRV, as common risk factors for CV disease, would moderate the effect. We presented a subliminal semantic priming paradigm to college students that were randomly assigned to the threat (n = 56) or neutral condition (n = 60) and assessed changes from baseline of MAP, TPR, and HRV. Level of trait worry was assessed with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. The findings indicate that CV activity changed according to the hypothesized pattern: A higher MAP and TPR and a lower HRV in the threat condition compared with the neutral condition were found with practically meaningful effect sizes. However, these findings were only statistically significant for TPR. Furthermore, changes in CV activity were not moderated by trait worry or resting HRV. This is the first study to explicitly address the role of subliminally presented threat words on health-relevant outcome measures and suggests that unconscious stress can influence peripheral vascular resistance.
REFERENCES
- Batelaan, N. M., Ten Have, M., Van Balkom, A. J. L. M., Tuithof, M., & De Graaf, R. (2014), Anxiety disorders and onset of cardiovascular disease: The differential impact of panic, phobias and worry. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(2), 252–258. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.12.003
- Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2014). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 78(5), 651–660. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5
- Behar, E., Alcaine, O., Zuellig, A. R., & Borkovec, T. D. (2003). Screening for generalized anxiety disorder using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire: A receiver operating characteristic analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34(1), 25–43. doi:10.1016/S0005-7916(03)00004-1
- Blascovich, J. (2008). Challenge and threat. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 431–445). NewYork, NY: Psychology Press.
- Borkovec, T. D., Ray, W. J., & Stöber, J. (1998). Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22(6), 561–576. doi:10.1023/A:1018790003416
- Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Technical Report C-1). Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
- Brosschot, J. F. (2010). Markers of chronic stress: Prolonged physiological activation and (un)conscious perseverative cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 46–50. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.004
- Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60(2), 113–124. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
- Brosschot, J. F., Verkuil, B., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). Conscious and unconscious perseverative cognition: Is a large part of prolonged physiological activity due to unconscious stress? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 69(4), 407–416. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.02.002
- Chalmers, J. A., Heathers, J. A. J., Abbott, M. J., Kemp, A. H., & Quintana, D. S. (2016). Worry is associated with robust reductions in heart rate variability: A transdiagnostic study of anxiety psychopathology. BMC Psychology, 4(1), 32. doi:10.1186/s40359-016-0138-z
- Chatelain, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2015). Implicit fear and effort-related cardiac response. Biological Psychology, 111, 73–82. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.08.009
- Codispoti, M., Mazzetti, M., & Bradley, M. M. (2009). Unmasking emotion: Exposure duration and emotional engagement. Psychophysiology, 46(4), 731–738. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00804.x
- Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Neubauer, D. H., Mauss, I. B., & Gross, J. J. (2002). Implicit anxiety measure predicts cardiovascular reactivity to an evaluated speaking task. Emotion, 2(1), 3–11. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.2.1.3
- Fagard, R. H., Pardaens, K., Staessen, J. A., & Thijs, L. (1996). Prognostic value of invasive hemodynamic measurements at rest and during exercise in hypertensive men. Hypertension, 28(1), 31–36. doi:10.1161/01.hyp.28.1.31
- Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. doi:10.1080/0269993004200024
- Frasure-Smith, N., & Lesperance, F. (2008). Depression and anxiety as predictors of 2-year cardiac events in patients with stable coronary artery disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(1), 62–71. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.4
- Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Mennin, D. S., & Turk, C. L. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research in Therapy, 40(3), 313–323. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00113-3
- Garfinkel, S. N., Zorab, E., Navaratnam, N., Engels, M., Mallorquí-Bagué, N., Minati, L., … Critchley, H. D. (2016). Anger in brain and body: The neural and physiological perturbation of decision-making by emotion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 150–158. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv099
- Gerin, W., Davidson, K. W., Christenfeld, N. J., Goyal, T., & Schwartz, J. E. (2006). The role of angry rumination and distraction in blood pressure recovery from emotional arousal. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(1), 64–72. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000195747.12404.aa
- Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Relationship between heart rate variability and cognitive function during threat of shock. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 22(1), 77–89. doi:10.1080/10615800802272251
- Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Hayes, S., Hirsch, C. R., Krebs, G., & Mathews, A. (2010). The effects of modifying interpretation bias on worry in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 171–178. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.10.006
- Herring, D. R., White, K. R., Jabeen, L. N., Hinojos, M., Terrazas, G., Reyes, S. M., … Crites, S. L., Jr. (2013). On the automatic activation of attitudes: A quarter century of evaluative priming research. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1062–1089. doi:10.1037/a0031309
- Hill, L., Sollers, I. J., & Thayer, J. (2011). Evaluation of a simple estimation method for the derivation of cardiac output from arterial blood pressure and heart rate. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation, 48, 165–170.
- Hill, L., Sollers, I. J., & Thayer, J. (2012). Resistance reconstructed estimation of total peripheral resistance from computationally derived cardiac output. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation, 49, 216–223.
- Hirsch, C. R., & Mathews, A. (2012). A cognitive model of pathological worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(10), 636–646. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.06.007
- Hull, J. G., Slone, L. B., Meteyer, K. B., & Matthews, A. R. (2002). The nonconsciousness of self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 406–424. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.2.406
- Jönsson, P., & Sonnby-Borgström, M. (2003). The effects of pictures of emotional faces on tonic and phasic autonomic cardiac control in women and men. Biological Psychology, 62(2), 157–173. doi:10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00114-x
- Key, B. L., Campbell, T. S., Bacon, S. L., & Gerin, W. (2008). The influence of trait and state rumination on cardiovascular recovery from a negative emotional stressor. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31(3), 237–248. doi:10.1007/s10865-008-9152-9
- Knepp, M. M., & Friedman, B. H. (2008). Cardiovascular activity during laboratory tasks in women with high and low worry. Biological Psychology, 79(3), 287–293. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.07.002
- Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857–875. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2093
- Kubzansky, L. D., Kawachi, I., Spiro, A., Weiss, S. T., Vokonas, P. S., & Sparrow, D. (1997). Is worrying bad for your heart? A prospective study of worry and coronary heart disease in the Normative Aging Study. Circulation, 95(4), 818–824. doi:10.1161/01.cir.95.4.818
- Lasauskaite, R., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2013). Do sadness-primes make me work harder because they make me sad? Cognition and Emotion, 27(1), 158–165. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.689756
- Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age by implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1092–1107. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1092
-
Levy, B. R.,
Hausdorff, J. M.,
Hencke, R., &
Wei, J. Y. (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55B(4), P205–P213. doi:10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205
10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205 Google Scholar
- Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measures of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28(6), 701–711. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01017.x
- Macmillan, N. A & Creelman, C. D. (2005) Detection theory: A user's guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Inc.
- Malik, M., Bigger, J. T., Camm, A. J., Kleiger, R. E., Malliani, A., Moss, A. J., & Schwartz, P. J. (1996). Heart rate variability standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. European Heart Journal, 17(3), 354–381. doi:10.4236/jbbs.2013.31004
- Martens, E. J., de Jonge, P., Na, B., Cohen, B. E., Lett, H., & Whooley, M. A. (2010). Scared to death? Generalized anxiety disorder and cardiovascular events in patients with stable coronary heart disease: The Heart and Soul Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(7), 750–758. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.74
- Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 563–569. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90104-4
- Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916
- Meerman, E. E., Verkuil, B., & Brosschot, J. F. (2011). Decreasing pain tolerance outside of awareness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 70(3), 250–257. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.08.006
- Merikle, P. (1984). Toward a definition of awareness. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(5), 449–450. doi:10.3758/BF03333874
- Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487–495. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6
- Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire: Psychometric properties and associated characteristics. In G. C. L. Davey, F. Tallis, G. C. L. Davey, & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment, and treatment. (pp. 265–283). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.
- Nielsen, L., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2006). Awareness of subtle emotional feelings: A comparison of long-term meditators and nonmeditators. Emotion, 6(3), 392–405. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.392
- Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). 'Unconscious anxiety': Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(2), 231–240. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.231
- Ottaviani, C., Brosschot, J. F., & Thayer, J. F. (2016, March). Hemodynamic profiles of adaptive and maladaptive forms of perseverative cognitions: Implications for health. In J. F. Brosschot & J. F. Thayer (Chairs), When thoughts can break your heart: Mechanisms linking perseverative cognition to health risk. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychosomatic Society, Denver, CO.
- Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F. (2016). Physiological concomitants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142(3), 231–259. doi:10.1037/bul0000036
- Pappens, M., Schroijen, M., Sutterlin, S., Smets, E., Van den Bergh, O., Thayer, J. F., & Van Diest, I. (2014). Resting heart rate variability predicts safety learning and fear extinction in an interoceptive fear conditioning paradigm. PLOS ONE, 9(9), 9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0105054
- Park, G., & Thayer, J. F. (2014). From the heart to the mind: Cardiac vagal tone modulates top-down and bottom-up visual perception and attention to emotional stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00278
- Pieper, S., Brosschot, J. F., van der Leeden, R., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). Prolonged cardiac effects of momentary assessed stressful events and worry episodes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(6), 570–577. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181dbc0e9
- Plante, G. E. (2002). Vascular response to stress in health and disease. Metabolism, 51(6), 25–30. doi:10.1053/meta.2002.33187
- Quirin, M., Kazen, M., & Kuhl, J. (2009). When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(3), 500–516. doi:10.1037/a0016063
- Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 510–532. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510
- Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis ( 2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
-
Rucker, D. D.,
Preacher, K. J.,
Tormala, Z. L., &
Petty, R. E. (2011). Mediation analysis in social psychology: Current practices and new recommendations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(6), 359–371. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.x
10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.x Google Scholar
- Seery, M. D. (2011). Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews ( 35), 1603–1610. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.003
- Shapiro, D., Jamner, L. D., Lane, J. D., Light, K. C., Myrtek, M., Sawada, Y., & Steptoe, A. (1996). Blood pressure publication guidelines. Psychophysiology, 33(1), 1–12. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02103.x
- Sherwood, A., Allen, M. T., Fahrenberg, J., Kelsey, R. M., Lovallo, W. R., & Vandoornen, L. J. P. (1990). Methodological guidelines for impedence cardiography. Psychophysiology, 27(1), 1–23. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02171.x
- Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. (2011). Do not prime too much: Prime frequency effects of masked affective stimuli on effort-related cardiovascular response. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 195–199. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.01.006
- Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Everaert, T., & Hermans, D. (2012). Unconscious semantic activation depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition, 122(1), 91–95. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.017
- Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93, 1043–1065. doi:10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043
- Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., & Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 747–756. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009
- Thayer, J. F., Friedman, B. H., & Borkovec, T. D. (1996). Autonomic characteristics of generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry, 39(4), 255–266. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00136-0
- Thayer, J. F., Friedman, B. H., Borkovec, T. D., Johnsen, B. H., & Molina, S. (2000). Phasic heart period reactions to cued threat and nonthreat stimuli in generalized anxiety disorder. Psychophysiology, 37(3), 361–368. doi:10.1017/s0048577200982192
- Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2007). The role of vagal function in the risk for cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 224–242. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013
- Tully, P. J., Cosh, S. M., & Baune, B. T. (2013). A review of the affects of worry and generalized anxiety disorder upon cardiovascular health and coronary heart disease. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 18(6), 627–644. doi:10.1080/13548506.2012.749355
- Van den Bussche, E., Van den Noortgate, W., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 452–477. doi:10.1037/a0015329
- Van den Hout, M., Tenney, N., Huygens, K., Merckelbach, H., & Kindt, M. (1995). Responding to subliminal threat cues is related to trait anxiety and emotional vulnerability: A successful replication of Macleod and Hagan (1992). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(4), 451–454. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00062-O
- Van der Ploeg, M. M., Brosschot, J. F., Thayer, J. F., & Verkuil, B. (2016). The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect test: Validity and relationship with cardiovascular stress-responses. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00425
- Van der Ploeg, M. M., Brosschot, J. F., Versluis, A., & Verkuil, B. (2016). Peripheral physiological responses to stressful stimuli outside of awareness: A systematic review. Manuscript submitted for publication.
-
Van der Ploeg, M. M.,
Koenig, J. K.,
Tarvainen, M. P., &
Thayer, J. F. (2017). From individual output to pooled data: A post-processing macro for Kubios HRV 2.2. Journal of Psychophysiology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000196
10.1027/0269-8803/a000196 Google Scholar
- Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., De Beurs, D. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Effects of explicit and implicit perseverative cognition on cardiac recovery after cognitive stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74(3), 220–228. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.003
-
Verkuil, B.,
Brosschot, J. F.,
Gebhardt, W. A., &
Thayer, J. F. (2010). When worries make you sick: A review of perseverative cognition, the default stress response and somatic health. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 1(1), 87–118. doi:10.5127/jep.009110
10.5127/jep.009110 Google Scholar
- Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., & Thayer, J. F. (2014). Cardiac reactivity to and recovery from acute stress: Temporal associations with implicit anxiety. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 92(2), 85–91. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.03.002
- Weber, C. S., Thayer, J. F., Rudat, M., Wirtz, P. H., Zimmermann-Viehoff, F., Thomas, A., … Deter, H. C. (2010). Low vagal tone is associated with impaired post stress recovery of cardiovascular, endocrine, and immune markers. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 109(2), 201–211. doi:10.1007/s00421-009-1341-x
- Wendt, J., Neubert, J., Koenig, J., Thayer, J. F., & Hamm, A. O. (2015). Resting heart rate variability is associated with inhibition of conditioned fear. Psychophysiology, 52(9), 1161–1166. doi:10.1111/psyp.12456
-
Williams, D. P.,
Cash, C.,
Rankin, C.,
Bernardi, A.,
Koenig, J., &
Thayer, J. F. (2015) Resting heart rate variability predicts self-reported difficulties in emotion regulation: A focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261 Google Scholar
-
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), S1–S17. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151
10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151 Google Scholar