Attentional abilities constrain language development: A cross-syndrome infant/toddler study
Corresponding Author
Dean D'Souza
Faculty of Science and Engineering, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, UK
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Correspondence
Dean D'Souza, Anglia Ruskin University, East Road, Cambridge, CB1 1PT, UK.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorHana D'Souza
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Department of Psychology & Newnham College, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Search for more papers by this authorEmily J. H. Jones
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Search for more papers by this authorAnnette Karmiloff-Smith
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Dean D'Souza
Faculty of Science and Engineering, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, UK
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Correspondence
Dean D'Souza, Anglia Ruskin University, East Road, Cambridge, CB1 1PT, UK.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorHana D'Souza
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Department of Psychology & Newnham College, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Search for more papers by this authorEmily J. H. Jones
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Search for more papers by this authorAnnette Karmiloff-Smith
Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, UK
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Typically developing (TD) infants adapt to the social world in part by shifting the focus of their processing resources to the relevant aspects of a visual scene. Any impairment in visual orienting may therefore constrain learning and development in domains such as language. However, although something is known about visual orienting in infants at risk of autism, very little is known about it in infants/toddlers with other neurodevelopmental disorders. This is partly because previous studies focused on older children and rarely compared the children to both chronological age (CA)- and mental age (MA)-matched TD controls. Yet, if visual orienting is important for learning and development, then it is imperative to investigate it early in development and ascertain whether it relates to higher level cognitive functions such as language. We used eye-tracking technology to directly compare visual orienting in infants/toddlers with one of three neurodevelopmental disorders—Down syndrome (DS), fragile X syndrome (FXS) and Williams syndrome (WS)—matched on CA or MA to TD controls (~15 months). We also measured language ability using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). We found that the ability to disengage attention from a visual stimulus in order to shift it to another visual stimulus is related to language ability in infants/toddlers irrespective of group affiliation. We also found that, contrary to the literature, infants and toddlers with DS (but not WS) are slow at disengaging attention. Our data suggest that orienting attention constrains language development and is impaired in DS.
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
The authors report no conflict of interest.
Open Research
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
desc12961-sup-0001-FigS1.pngPNG image, 26.8 KB | Fig S1 |
desc12961-sup-0002-FigS2.pngPNG image, 12.7 KB | Fig S2 |
desc12961-sup-0003-FigS3.pngPNG image, 56.8 KB | Fig S3 |
desc12961-sup-0004-FigS4.pngPNG image, 15.1 KB | Fig S4 |
desc12961-sup-0005-FigS5.pngPNG image, 17.9 KB | Fig S5 |
desc12961-sup-0006-FigS6.pngPNG image, 48.9 KB | Fig S6 |
desc12961-sup-0007-FigS7.pngPNG image, 50.9 KB | Fig S7 |
desc12961-sup-0008-Supinfo.docxWord document, 142.8 KB | Supplementary Material |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
REFERENCES
- Alcock, K. J., & Krawczyk, K. (2010). Individual differences in language development: Relationship with motor skill at 21 months. Developmental Science, 13(5), 677–691.
- Anderson, J. S., Nielsen, J. A., Ferguson, M. A., Burback, M. C., Cox, E. T., Dai, L. I., … Korenberg, J. R. (2013). Abnormal brain synchrony in down syndrome. NeuroImage: Clinical, 2, 703–715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.006
- Ansado, J., Collins, L., Fonov, V., Garon, M., Alexandrov, L., Karama, S., … Beauchamp, M. H. (2015). A new template to study callosal growth shows specific growth in anterior and posterior regions of the corpus callosum in early childhood. European Journal of Neuroscience, 42(1), 1675–1684. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12869
- Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. J. (2011). From genes to brain development to phenotypic behavior: "dorsal-stream vulnerability" in relation to spatial cognition, attention, and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental disorders. Progress in Brain Research, 189, 261–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53884-0.00029-4
- Atkinson, J., Braddick, O. J., Anker, S., Curran, W., Andrew, R., Wattam-Bell, J., & Braddick, F. (2003). Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: Measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1–2), 139–172. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN231&2_7
- Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J. T., & Soska, K. C. (2018). The Multisensory Attention Assessment Protocol (MAAP): Characterizing individual differences in multisensory attention skills in infants and children and relations with language and cognition. Developmental Psychology, 54(12), 2207. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000594
- Bates, E. (Ed.). (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press.
- Breckenridge, K., Braddick, O., Anker, S., Woodhouse, M., & Atkinson, J. (2012). Attention in Williams syndrome and Down’s syndrome: Performance on the new early childhood attention battery. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 257–269. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12003
- Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.
- Brown, J. H., Johnson, M. H., Paterson, S. J., Gilmore, R., Longhi, E., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Spatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 1037–1046. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00299-3
- Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
- Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–174. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166214
- Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
- Cornish, K. M., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672–685. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70497-0
- D’Souza, D. (2014). Are early cognitive and neurophysiological markers of autism syndrome-specific? A cross-syndrome comparison (Unpublished doctoral thesis). London, UK: University of London.
- D'Souza, D., Brady, D., Haensel, J.X., & D'Souza, H. (2020). Is mere exposure enough? The effects of bilingual environments on infant cognitive development. Royal Society Open Science, 7(2), 180191.
- D’Souza, D., D’Souza, H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Precursors to language development in typically and atypically developing infants and toddlers: The importance of embracing complexity. Journal of Child Language, 44(3), 591–627. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091700006X
- Desimone, R. (1998). Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London—Series B Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1245–1255.
- Driver, J., Eimer, M., Macaluso, E., & van Velzen, J. (2004). Neurobiology of human spatial attention: Modulation, generation, and integration. In N. Kanwisher & J. Duncan (Eds.), Attention and performance XX: Functional brain imaging of visual cognition (pp. 267–300). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
- Elison, J. T., Paterson, S. J., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Sasson, N. J., Gu, H., … Gerig, G. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 899–908.
- Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Jane Webb, S., Dawson, G., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 189–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030
- Fidler, D. J. (2005). The emerging Down syndrome behavioral phenotype in early childhood. Infants & Young Children, 18(2), 86–103. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200504000-00003
- Flanagan, T., Enns, J. T., Murphy, M. M., Russo, N., Abbeduto, L., Randolph, B., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Differences in visual orienting between persons with Down or fragile X syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 128–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.01.009
- Goodwin, A., Salomone, S., Bolton, P., Charman, T., Jones, E. J. H., Pickles, A., … Johnson, M. H. (2016). Attention training for infants at familial risk of ADHD (INTERSTAARS): Study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials, 17(1), 608. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-016-1727-0
- Green, J., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Wan, M. W., Elsabbagh, M., Slonims, V., … Johnson, M. H. (2015). Parent-mediated intervention versus no intervention for infants at high risk of autism: A parallel, single-blind, randomised trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(2), 133–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00091-1
- Griffin, I. C., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). Orienting attention to locations in internal representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1176–1194. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903322598139
- Hahn, L. J., Loveall, S. J., Savoy, M. T., Neumann, A. M., & Ikuta, T. (2018). Joint attention in Down syndrome: A meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 78, 89–102.
- Harris, S., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. D. (1996). Joint attention and language gains in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100(6), 608–619.
- Hayhoe, M., & Ballard, D. (2005). Eye movements in natural behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 188–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.009
- Hood, B. M., & Atkinson, J. (1993). Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 405–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(93)80001-O
- Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult's eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00024
- Iacobucci, D., Posavac, S. S., Kardes, F. R., Schneider, M. J., & Popovich, D. L. (2015). The median split: Robust, refined, and revived. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(4), 690–704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.06.014
- Irving, E. L., Steinbach, M. J., Lillakas, L., Babu, R. J., & Hutchings, N. (2006). Horizontal saccade dynamics across the human life span. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 47(6), 2478–2484. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.05-1311
- Jackowski, A. P., Rando, K., de Araújo, C. M., Del Cole, C. G., Silva, I., & de Lacerda, A. L. T. (2009). Brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome: A review of structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging findings. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 13(4), 305–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2008.07.002
- Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(4), 335–344. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.335
- Kulke, L., Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. (2017). Neural mechanisms of attention become more specialised during infancy: Insights from combined eye tracking and EEG. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(2), 250–260.
- Kucker, S. C., McMurray, B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2015). Slowing down fast mapping: Redefining the dynamics of word learning. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 74–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12110
- Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., … Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5(2), 233–246.
- Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(6), 1115–1122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00304.x
- Leonard, H. C., & Hill, E. L. (2014). The impact of motor development on typical and atypical social cognition and language: A systematic review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 19(3), 163–170.
- Matsuzawa, M., & Shimojo, S. (1997). Infants' fast saccades in the gap paradigm and development of visual attention. Infant Behavior & Development, 20(4), 449–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90035-7
- Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS edn.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
- Mundy, P., Slgman, M., Kasarl, C., & Yirmiya, N. (1988). Nonverbal communication skills in Down Syndrome children. Child Development, 59, 235–249.
- Niedźwiecka, A., Ramotowska, S., & Tomalski, P. (2018). Mutual gaze during early mother–infant interactions promotes attention control development. Child Development, 89(6), 2230–2244. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12830
- Pereira, A. F., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2014). A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(1), 178–185. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0466-4
- Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42.
- Quine, W. V. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2009). Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or Autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 421–431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z
- Riby, D. M., Jones, N., Brown, P. H., Robinson, L. J., Langton, S. R., Bruce, V., & Riby, L. M. (2011). Attention to faces in Williams syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(9), 1228–1239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1141-5
- Roberts, J. E., Hatton, D. D., Long, A. C., Anello, V., & Colombo, J. (2012). Visual attention and autistic behavior in infants with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 937–946. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1316-8
- Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266. https://doi.org/10.1038/253265a0
- Slaughter, V., & McConnell, D. (2003). Emergence of joint attention: Relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(1), 54–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221320309597503
- Stevens, C., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), S30–S48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.11.001
- Summerfield, J. J., Lepsien, J., Gitelman, D. R., Mesulam, M., & Nobre, A. C. (2006). Orienting attention based on long-term memory experience. Neuron, 49(6), 905–916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.021
- Takikawa, Y., Kawagoe, R., Itoh, H., Nakahara, H., & Hikosaka, O. (2002). Modulation of saccadic eye movements by predicted reward outcome. Experimental Brain Research, 142(2), 284–291. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-001-0928-1
- Venker, C. E. (2017). Spoken word recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder: The role of visual disengagement. Autism, 21(7), 821–829. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316653230
- Wang, M. V., Lekhal, R., Aaro, L. E., Holte, A., & Schjolberg, S. (2014). The developmental relationship between language and motor performance from 3 to 5 years of age: A prospective longitudinal population study. BMC Psychology, 2(1), 34.
10.1186/s40359-014-0034-3 Google Scholar
- Yazbek, A., & D'Entremont, B. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of the still-face effect at 6 months and joint attention at 12 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 589–601.
- Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125(2), 244–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.016
- Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Joint attention without gaze following: Human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e79659. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079659
- Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2015). Linking joint attention with hand-eye coordination–a sensorimotor approach to understanding child-parent social interactio. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (Vol. 2015, p. 2763). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Retrieved from https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2015/papers/0473/paper0473.pdf
- Yu, C., & Smith, L.B. (2017). Hand–eye coordination predicts joint attention. Child Development, 88(6), 2060–2078.
- Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001