A second type of magnitude effect: Reinforcer magnitude differentiates delay discounting between substance users and controls
Alexandra M. Mellis
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
Graduate Program in Translational Biology, Medicine, and HEALTH, VIRGINIA TECH
Search for more papers by this authorAlina E. Woodford
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
University of Virginia
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Warren K. Bickel
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Warren K. Bickel, Ph.D., Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, 2 Riverside Circle, Roanoke, VA 24016. Email: [email protected], 540-526-2088Search for more papers by this authorAlexandra M. Mellis
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
Graduate Program in Translational Biology, Medicine, and HEALTH, VIRGINIA TECH
Search for more papers by this authorAlina E. Woodford
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
University of Virginia
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Warren K. Bickel
Virginia Tech–Carilion Research Institute
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Warren K. Bickel, Ph.D., Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, 2 Riverside Circle, Roanoke, VA 24016. Email: [email protected], 540-526-2088Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Basic research on delay discounting, examining preference for smaller–sooner or larger–later reinforcers, has demonstrated a variety of findings of considerable generality. One of these, the magnitude effect, is the observation that individuals tend to exhibit greater preference for the immediate with smaller magnitude reinforcers. Delay discounting has also proved to be a useful marker of addiction, as demonstrated by the highly replicated finding of greater discounting rates in substance users compared to controls. However, some research on delay discounting rates in substance users, particularly research examining discounting of small-magnitude reinforcers, has not found significant differences compared to controls. Here, we hypothesize that the magnitude effect could produce ceiling effects at small magnitudes, thus obscuring differences in delay discounting between groups. We examined differences in discounting between high-risk substance users and controls over a broad range of magnitudes of monetary amounts ($0.10, $1.00, $10.00, $100.00, and $1000.00) in 116 Amazon Mechanical Turk workers. We found no significant differences in discounting rates between users and controls at the smallest reinforcer magnitudes ($0.10 and $1.00) and further found that differences became more pronounced as magnitudes increased. These results provide an understanding of a second form of the magnitude effect: That is, differences in discounting between populations can become more evident as a function of reinforcer magnitude.
References
- Amlung, M., Petker, T., Jackson, J., Balodis, I., & MacKillop, J. (2016). Steep discounting of delayed monetary and food rewards in obesity: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(11), 2423–2434. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000866
- Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), 382–392. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12943017
- Bickel, W. K., George Wilson, A., Franck, C. T., Terry Mueller, E., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Koffarnus, M. N., & Fede, S. J. (2014). Using crowdsourcing to compare temporal, social temporal, and probability discounting among obese and non-obese individuals. Appetite, 75, 82–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.018
- Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., Koffarnus, M. N., & Gatchalian, K. M. (2012). Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 134(3), 287–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2012.02.004
- Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/09652140020016978
- Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00005490
- Bickel, W. K., Wilson, A. G., Chen, C., Koffarnus, M. N., & Franck, C. T. (2016). Stuck in time: Negative income shock constricts the temporal window of valuation spanning the future and the past. PloS One.
- Bongers, P., van de Giessen, E., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Booij, J., van den Brink, W., & Jansen, A. (2015). Being impulsive and obese increases susceptibility to speeded detection of high-calorie foods. Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 34(6), 677–685. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000167
- Bradford, W. D. (2010). The association between individual time preferences and health maintenance habits. Medical Decision Making: An International Journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making, 30(1), 99–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342276
- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the social sciences ( 2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Dixon, M. R., Marley, J., & Jacobs, E. A. (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(4), 449–458.
- Epstein, L. H., Salvy, S. J., Carr, K. A., Dearing, K. K., & Bickel, W. K. (2010). Food reinforcement, delay discounting and obesity. Physiology & Behavior, 100(5), 438–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.04.029
- Fagerstrom, K. (2012). Determinants of tobacco use and renaming the FTND to the Fagerstrom Test for Cigarette Dependence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco, 14(1), 75–78. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntr137
- Fields, S. A., Sabet, M., & Reynolds, B. (2013). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in obese, overweight, and healthy-weight adolescents. Appetite, 70, 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.089
- Fields, S., Leraas, K., Collins, C., & Reynolds, B. (2009). Delay discounting as a mediator of the relationship between perceived stress and cigarette smoking status in adolescents. Behavioural Pharmacology, 20(5–6), 455–460. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e328330dcff
- Grace, R. C., & McLean, A. P. (2005). Integrated versus segregated accounting and the magnitude effect in temporal discounting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 732–739. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16447389
- Green, L., Myerson, J., & McFadden, E. (1997). Rate of temporal discounting decreases with amount of reward. Memory & Cognition, 25(5), 715–723. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9337589
- Green, L., Myerson, J., Oliveira, L., & Chang, S. E. (2013). Delay discounting of monetary rewards over a wide range of amounts. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 100(3), 269–281. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.45
- Greenhouse, S.W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psykometrika, 24, 95–112.
- Heil, S. H., Johnson, M. W., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (2006). Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addictive Behaviors, 31(7), 1290–1294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.09.005
- Hendrickson, K. L., & Rasmussen, E. B. (2013). Effects of mindful eating training on delay and probability discounting for food and money in obese and healthy-weight individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(7), 399–409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.04.002
- Hoffman, W. F., Moore, M., Templin, R., McFarland, B., Hitzemann, R. J., & Mitchell, S. H. (2006). Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology, 188(2), 162–170. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0494-0
- Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77(2), 129–146. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.77-129
- Johnson, M. W., Bickel, W. K., & Baker, F. (2007). Moderate drug use and delay discounting: a comparison of heavy, light, and never smokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15(2), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.15.2.187
- Johnson, P. S., Herrmann, E. S., & Johnson, M. W. (2015). Opportunity costs of reward delays and the discounting of hypothetical money and cigarettes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103(1), 87–107. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.110
- Kirby, K. N., & Petry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction, 99(4), 461–471. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x
- Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 128(1), 78–87. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10100392
- Koffarnus, M. N., & Bickel, W. K. (2014). A 5-trial adjusting delay discounting task: accurate discount rates in less than one minute. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22(3), 222–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035973
- MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Few, L. R., Ray, L. A., Sweet, L. H., & Munafò, M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology, 216(3), 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0
- MacKillop, J., Miranda, R., Jr., Monti, P. M., Ray, L. A., Murphy, J. G., Rohsenow, D. J., … Gwaltney, C. J. (2010). Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 106–114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017513
- Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., & Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(2), 139–145. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12755458
- Madden, G. J., Petry, N. M., & Johnson, P. S. (2009). Pathological gamblers discount probabilistic rewards less steeply than matched controls. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17(5), 283–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016806
- Mauchly, J. W. (1940). Significance test for sphericity of a normal n-variate distribution. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 5, 269–287.
- Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M.L. Commons, J.E. Mazur, J.A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events of reinforcement value, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 55–73.
- Mejía-Cruz, D., Green, L., Myerson, J., Morales-Chainé, S., & Nieto, J. (2016). Delay and probability discounting by drug-dependent cocaine and marijuana users. Psychopharmacology, 233(14), 2705–2714. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4316-8
- Mitchell, J. M., Fields, H. L., D'Esposito, M., & Boettiger, C. A. (2005). Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(12), 2158–2169. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16385186
- Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 455–464. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10550496
- Moody, L., Franck, C., Hatz, L., & Bickel, W. K. (2016). Impulsivity and polysubstance use: A systematic comparison of delay discounting in mono-, dual-, and trisubstance use. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 24(1), 30–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000059
- Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: I'm a k, you're a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96(3), 427–439. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423
- Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., & Kitamura, N. (2005). Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology, 182(4), 508–515. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8
- Petry, N. M. (2001). Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology, 154(3), 243–250. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11351931
- Petry, N. M. (2002). Discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers: Relationship to antisocial personality disorder. Psychopharmacology, 162(4), 425–432. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-002-1115-1
- Petry, N. M. (2003). Discounting of money, health, and freedom in substance abusers and controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 71(2), 133–141. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12927651
- Piccinelli, M. (1998). Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT). Epidemiologia E Psichiatria Sociale, 7(01), 70–73. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1121189X00007144
- Reynolds, B., Karraker, K., Horn, K., & Richards, J. B. (2003). Delay and probability discounting as related to different stages of adolescent smoking and non-smoking. Behavioural Processes, 64(3), 333–344. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14580702
- Reynolds, B., Patak, M., Shroff, P., Penfold, R. B., Melanko, S., & Duhig, A. M. (2007). Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behavior in adolescent daily smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15(3), 264.
- Vuchinich, R. E., & Simpson, C. A. (1999). Delayed-reward discounting in alcohol abuse. In F.J. Chaloupka, M. Grossman, W.K. Bickel, & H. Saffer (Eds.), The economic analysis of substance use and abuse. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 103–122.
- Wilson, A. G., Franck, C. T., Koffarnus, M. N., & Bickel, W. K. (2016). Behavioral economics of cigarette purchase tasks: Within-subject comparison of real, potentially real, and hypothetical cigarettes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco, 18(5), 524–530. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntv154
- Yoon, J. H., & Higgins, S. T. (2008). Turning k on its head: Comments on use of an ED50 in delay discounting research. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 95(1–2), 169–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.011