The effect of construal level on risk-taking
Corresponding Author
Eva Lermer
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Correspondence to: Eva Lermer, Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Leopoldstr. 13, Munich, Germany 80803, Germany.
E-mail: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorBernhard Streicher
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorMartina Raue
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorDieter Frey
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Eva Lermer
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Correspondence to: Eva Lermer, Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Leopoldstr. 13, Munich, Germany 80803, Germany.
E-mail: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorBernhard Streicher
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorMartina Raue
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorDieter Frey
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
In a series of studies, we examined the influence of people's mind-set (construal level (CL): abstract versus concrete) on their risk-taking behavior. We measured differences in CL (study 1, CL as trait) and manipulated CL (studies 1–5, CL as state) with different priming methods, which were unrelated to the dependent variable of risk-taking behavior (studies 1, 3, 4, and 5: Balloon Analog Risk Task; study 2: Angling Risk Task). In all studies, abstract CL resulted in greater risk-taking compared with concrete CL, which led to lower risk-taking. Risky and safe game strategies mediated the CL effect on risk-taking. A concrete mind-set increased the safe game strategy, whereas an abstract mind-set increased the risky game strategy. Furthermore, different potential mediators were explored (i.e., focus on payoffs and probabilities, prevention versus promotion focus, attention to pros versus cons, and mood). A concrete mind-set increased prevention strategies and a negative mood when compared with an abstract mind-set. In turn, an abstract mind-set increased attention to pros (of an action). Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
ejsp2067-sup-0001-Supplementary.docxWord 2007 document , 17.3 KB | Supporting info item |
ejsp2067-sup-0002-Supplementary1.savapplication/unknown, 4.2 KB | Supporting info item |
ejsp2067-sup-0003-Supplementary2.savapplication/unknown, 2.1 KB | Supporting info item |
ejsp2067-sup-0004-Supplementary3.savapplication/unknown, 3.9 KB | Supporting info item |
ejsp2067-sup-0005-Supplementary4.savapplication/unknown, 4 KB | Supporting info item |
ejsp2067-sup-0006-Supplementary5.savapplication/unknown, 5.5 KB | Supporting info item |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2006). The association between psychological distance and construal level: Evidence from an implicit association test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 609–622. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.609
- Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
- Beckmann, J. (1993). Verhaltensidentifikations-Fragebogen. Eine deutsche Übersetzung der Behavioral Identification Form von Vallacher und Wegner [Behavior-identification-questionnaire. A German translation of the Behavioral Identification Form by Vallacher and Wegner]. Unpublished manuscript. Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany.
- Bell, N. J., Schoenrock, C. J., & O'Neal, K. K. (2000). Self-monitoring and the propensity for risk. European Journal of Personality, 14, 107–119. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(200003/04)14:2<107::AID-PER365>3.0.CO;2-T
- Blais, A. R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 33–47. doi: 10.1037/t13084-000
- Bryant, P., & Dunford, R. (2008). The influence of regulatory focus on risky decision-making. Applied Psychology, 57, 335–359. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00319.x
- Burgoon, E. M., Henderson, M. D., & Markman, A. B. (2013). There are many ways to see the forest for the trees: A tour guide for abstraction. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(5), 501–520. doi: 10.1177/1745691613497964
- Chandler, J. J., & Pronin, E. (2012). Fast thought speed induces risk taking. Psychological Science, 23, 370–374. doi: 10.1177/0956797611431464
- Crowe, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Regulatory focus and strategic inclinations: Promotion and prevention in decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 117–132. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.2675
- De Vries, M., Holland, R. W., & Witteman, C. L. M. (2008). In the winning mood: Affect in the Iowa gambling task. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 42–50.
- Ditto, P. H., Pizarro, D. A., Epstein, E. B., Jacobson, J. A., & MacDonald, T. K. (2006). Visceral influences on risk-taking behavior. Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 99–113. doi: 10.1002/bdm.520
- Eyal, T., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Walther, E. (2004). The pros and cons of temporally near and distant action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 781–795. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.781
- Eysenck, S. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1977). The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 57–68. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1977.tb01003.x
- Figner, B., & Weber, E. U. (2011). Who takes risks when and why? Determinants of risk taking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 211–216. doi: 10.1177/0963721411415790
- Förster, J., Friedman, R. S., & Liberman, N. (2004). Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: consequences for insight and creative cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 177. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.177
- Förster, J., & Higgins, T. (2005). How global versus local perception fits regulatory focus. Psychological Science, 16, 631–636. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01586.x
- Freeman, N., & Muraven, M. (2010). Self-control depletion leads to increased risk taking. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 175–181. doi: 10.1177/1948550609360421
- Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others' self-regulation efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 739–752. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003
- Frey, D., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (1996). Eine Theorie der gelernten Sorglosigkeit. [A theory of learned carelessness]. In H. Mandel (Ed.), Bericht über den 40. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie (pp. 604–611). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
- Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351
- Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00406
- Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Optimism, promotion pride, and prevention pride as predictors of quality of life. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(12), 1521–1532. doi: 10.1177/0146167203256919
- Grühn, D., Kotter-Grühn, D., & Röcke, C. (2010). Discrete affects across the adult lifespan: Evidence for multidimensionality and multidirectionality of affective experiences in young, middle-aged and older adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 492–500. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.003
- Hanoch, Y., Johnson, J., & Wilke, A. (2006). Domain specificity in experimental measures and participant recruitment: An application to risk-taking behavior. Psychological Science, 17, 300–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01702.x
- Henderson, M. D., Wakslak, C. J., Fujita, K., & Rohrbach, J. (2011). Construal level theory and spatial distance: Implications for mental representation, judgment, and behavior. Social Psychology, 42, 165–173. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000060
- Hiemer, J., & Abele, A. E. (2012). High power = motivation? Low power = situation? The impact of power, power stability and power motivation on risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 486–490. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.008
- Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280
- Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3–23. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.27
- Hunt, M. K., Hopko, D. R., Bare, R., Lejuez, C. W., & Robinson, E. V. (2005). Construct validity of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART): Associations with psychopathy and impulsivity. Assessment, 12, 416–428. doi: 10.1177/1073191105278740
- Johnson, J. G., Wilke, A., & Weber, E. U. (2004). Beyond a trait view of risk-taking: A domain-specific scale measuring risk perceptions, expected benefits, and perceived-risk attitude in German-speaking populations. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 35, 153–163.
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185
- Lee, A. Y., Keller, P. A., & Sternthal, B. (2010). Value from regulatory construal fit: The persuasive impact of fit between consumer goals and message concreteness. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(5), 735–747. doi: 10.1086/605591
- Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Pedulla, C. M. (2003). Evaluating the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 475–479. doi: 10.1016/S0140-1971(03)00036-8
- Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75–84. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75
- Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 523–534. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00013-6
- Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.5
- Luguri, J. B., Napier, J. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2012). Reconstruing intolerance: Abstract thinking reduces conservatives prejudice against nonnormative groups. Psychological Science, 23, 756–763. doi: 10.1177/0956797611433877
- Pennington, G. L., & Roese, N. J. (2003). Regulatory focus and temporal distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 563–576. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00058-1
- Pleskac, T. J. (2004). Evaluating cognitive sequential risk-taking models: Manipulations of the stochastic process. Doctoral dissertation, Graduate School of the University of Maryland. Retrieved March 18, 2014 from: http://drum.lib.umd.edu/bitstream/1903/1854/1/umi-umd-1849.pdf
- Rim, S., Uleman, J. S., & Trope, Y. (2009). Spontaneous trait inference and construal level theory: Psychological distance increases nonconscious trait thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1088–1097. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.015
- Sagristano, M. D., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2002). Time-dependent gambling: Odds now, money later. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 364–376. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.3.364
- Scholer, A. A., Zou, X., Fujita, K., Stroessner, S. J., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). When risk seeking becomes a motivational necessity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 215–231. doi: 10.1037/a0019715
- Spassova, G., & Lee, A. Y. (2008). Self-construal and temporal distance. Advances in Consumer Research, 35, 841–842.
- Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 742–752. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.742
- Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation and coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429–439. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.429
- Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440–463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963
- Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation of action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 660–671. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660
- Wakslak, C., & Trope, Y. (2009). The effect of construal level on subjective probability estimates. Psychological Science, 20, 52–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02250.x
- Werth, L., & Förster, J. (2007). How regulatory focus influences consumer behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 33–51. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.343
- Yuen, K. S. L., & Lee, T. M. C. (2003). Could mood state affect risk-taking decisions? Journal of Affective Disorders, 75, 11–18. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00022-8
- Zou, X., Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2014). In pursuit of progress: Promotion motivation and risk preference in the domain of gains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(2), 183. doi: 10.1037/a0035391
-
Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risky behavior. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/11555-000
10.1037/11555‐000 Google Scholar
- Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 999–1029. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00124