Discourse comprehension in autism spectrum disorder: Effects of working memory load and common ground
Jillian M. Schuh
Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology-FWC, Medical College of Wisconsin 9200 West Wisconsin Ave, Milwaukee, Wisconsin
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Inge-Marie Eigsti
Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology-FWC, Medical College of Wisconsin 9200 West Wisconsin Ave, Milwaukee, Wisconsin
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, U-1020 Storrs, Connecticut
Address for correspondence and reprints: Inge-Marie Eigsti, Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, U-1020; Storrs, CT 06269. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorDaniel Mirman
Department of Psychology, 3141 Chestnut St., Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Search for more papers by this authorJillian M. Schuh
Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology-FWC, Medical College of Wisconsin 9200 West Wisconsin Ave, Milwaukee, Wisconsin
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Inge-Marie Eigsti
Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology-FWC, Medical College of Wisconsin 9200 West Wisconsin Ave, Milwaukee, Wisconsin
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, U-1020 Storrs, Connecticut
Address for correspondence and reprints: Inge-Marie Eigsti, Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, U-1020; Storrs, CT 06269. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorDaniel Mirman
Department of Psychology, 3141 Chestnut St., Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Pragmatic language impairments are nearly universal in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Discourse requires that we monitor information that is shared or mutually known, called “common ground.” While many studies have examined the role of Theory of Mind (ToM) in such impairments, few have examined working memory (WM). Common ground impairments in ASD could reflect limitations in both WM and ToM. This study explored common ground use in youth ages 8–17 years with high-functioning ASD (n = 13) and typical development (n = 22); groups did not differ on age, gender, IQ, or standardized language. We tracked participants' eye movements while they performed a discourse task in which some information was known only to the participant (e.g., was privileged; a manipulation of ToM). In addition, the amount of privileged information varied (a manipulation of WM). All participants were slower to fixate the target when considering privileged information, and this effect was greatest during high WM load trials. Further, the ASD group was more likely to fixate competing (non-target) shapes. Predictors of fixation patterns included ASD symptomatology, language ability, ToM, and WM. Groups did not differ in ToM. Individuals with better WM fixated the target more rapidly, suggesting an association between WM capacity and efficient discourse. In addition to ToM knowledge, WM capacity constrains common ground representation and impacts pragmatic skills in ASD. Social impairments in ASD are thus associated with WM capacity, such that deficits in domain-general, nonsocial processes such as WM exert an influence during complex social interactions. Autism Res 2016, 9: 1340–1352. © 2016 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher's web-site:
Filename | Description |
---|---|
aur1632-sup-0001-supptable1.doc52 KB |
Table S1. Examples of Common and Preivileged Ground trials, and Filler trials. |
aur1632-sup-0002-supptable2.doc63 KB |
Table S2. Fixed effect parameter estimates and significant tests for growth curve analyses of target fixations (left columns) and competitor fixations (right columns). |
aur1632-sup-0003-supptable3.doc41 KB |
Table S3. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis of competitor fixations for linear (top panel) and quadratic terms (bottom panel). |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- Ackerman, B.P., & Silver, D. (1990). Children's understanding of private keys in referential communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50(2), 217–242.
- Ackerman, B.P., Szymanski, J., & Silver, D. (1990). Children's use of the common ground in interpreting ambiguous referential utterances. Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 234–245.
- Asberg, J. (2010). Patterns of language and discourse comprehension skills in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorders. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(6), 534–539. doi: SJOP822 [pii] 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00822.x [doi]
- Austin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 655. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655. eCollection 02014
- Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Do people with autism understand what causes emotion? Child Development, 62, 385–395.
- Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–36.
- Benson, J.E., Sabbagh, M.A., Carlson, S.M., & Zelazo, P.D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers' improvement from theory-of-mind training. Developmental Psychology, 49(9), 1615–1627. doi: 1610.1037/a0031056. Epub 0032012 Dec 0031017
- Bodner, K., Minshew, N., & Williams, D.L. (2009). The Pittsburgh Inference Test (PIT): A pilot study evaluating a measure of discourse processing in individuals with high-functioning autism. Paper presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Chicago, IL.
- Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(5), 893–900.
- Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism, 2, 325–344.
10.1177/1362361398024002 Google Scholar
- Carlson, S.M., White, R.E., & Davis-Unger, A. (2014). Evidence for a relation between executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 29, 1–16.
- Chandler, M.J., Greenspan, S., & Barenboim, C. (1974). Assessment and training of role-taking and referential communication skills in institutionalized emotionally disturbed children. Developmental Psychology, 10(4), 546–553.
- Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happe, F., & Wilson, D. (2011). What's in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the Theory of Mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 507–517. doi: 510.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.1011.1042. Epub 2010 Dec 1014.
- Clark, H. (1992). Arenas of language use. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
- Clark, H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 1–39.
- Constantino, J.N. (2003). Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
- Dahlgren, S., & Sandberg, A.D. (2008). Referential communication in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 12(4), 335–348.
- Dawson, G., & Fernald, M. (1987). Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(4), 487–498.
- de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I.M. (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Research, 3, 311–322. doi: 10.1002/aur.159
- de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I.M. (2015). The art of common ground: Emergence of a complex pragmatic language skill in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language, 24, 1–38.
- de Villiers, P.A., de Villiers, J.G., Coles-While, D., & Carpenter, L. (2009, October 31 to November 2, 2008). Acquisition of relevance implicatures in typically-developing children and children with autism. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD), Boston, MA.
- Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-4th Ed. (4th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
- Eigsti, I.M. (2011). Executive functions. In D. Fein (Ed.), Neuropsychology of Autism (pp. 185–204). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Eigsti, I.M., Schuh, J.M., Mencl, E., Schultz, R.T., & Paul, R. (2012). The neural underpinnings of prosody in autism. Child Neuropsychology, 18(6), 600–617.
- Epley, N., Morewedge, C.K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 760–768.
- Farrant, B., Fletcher, J., & Maybery, M. (2006). Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: Evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. Child Development, 77(6), 1842–1853.
- Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 346(1315), 97–104. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1994.0133
- Garcia-Perez, R.M., Lee, A., & Hobson, R.P. (2007). On intersubjective engagement in autism: A controlled study of nonverbal aspects of conversation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1310–1322. Epub 2006 Nov 1314.
- Gathercole, S.E., Hitch, G.J., Service, E., & Martin, A.J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 966–979.
- Gaulin, C.A., & Campbell, T.F. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(1 Pt 1), 55–64.
- Geller, E. (1991). The interplay between linguistic and social-cognitive knowledge in perspective-taking by autistic children. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 14(1), 23–44.
10.1177/152574018801400103 Google Scholar
- Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235–238.
- Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R.M., & Higgins, E.T. (1975). The development of referential communication skills. In F.E. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of Child Development Research (Vol. 4, pp. 305–345). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J.L. Morgan (Eds.), Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.
10.1163/9789004368811_003 Google Scholar
- Hanna, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M.K. (2004). Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive science, 28(1), 105–115.
- Hanna, J.E., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Trueswell, J.C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 43–61.
- Harris, P. (1996). Desires, beliefs, and language. In P. Carruthers & P.K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200–220). NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Horton, W.S., & Gerrig, R.J. (2005a). Conversational common ground and memory processes in language production. Discourse Processes, 40(1), 1–35.
- Horton, W.S., & Gerrig, R.J. (2005b). The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production. Cognition, 96(2), 127–142.
- Hollingshead, A.B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Department of Sociology.
10.1016/0148-9062(75)90418-0 Google Scholar
- James, T. (2002). Relations between pragmatic development and theory of mind in young children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
- Joseph, R.M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6(1), 137–155.
- Keysar, B., Barr, D.J., Balin, J.A., & Brauner, J.S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11(1), 32–38.
- Korkman, M., Kemp, S.L., & Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 331–354.
- Krauss, R.M., & Fussell, S.R. (1991). Perspective-taking in communication: Representations of others' knowledge in reference. Social Cognition, 9, 2–24.
- Landa, R.J., & Goldberg, M. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557–573.
- Levy, N.K., & Milgram, N. (2014). Cognitive contributions to theory of mind ability in children with a traumatic head injury. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 1–20.
- Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P.C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
- Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
- Loukusa, S., & Moilanen, I. (2009). Pragmatic inference abilities in individuals with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. A review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(4), 890–904.
- Loveland, K.A., Landry, S.H., Hughes, S.O., Hall, S.K., & McEvoy, R.E. (1988). Speech acts and the pragmatic deficits of autism. Journal of speech and hearing research, 31(4), 593–604.
- Marsh, D.T., Serafica, F.C., & Barenboim, C. (1980). Effect of perspective-taking training on interpersonal problem solving. Child Development, 51(1), 140–145.
- Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development, 78(6), 1744–1759.
- Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC Press.
- Mirman, D., Dixon, J.A., & Magnuson, J.S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 475–494. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.006
- Mirman, D., Yee, E., Blumstein, S.E., & Magnuson, J.S. (2011). Theories of spoken word recognition deficits in aphasia: Evidence from eye-tracking and computational modeling. Brain and Language, 117(2), 53–68.
- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J.A., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 349–364.
- Mutter, B., Alcorn, M.B., & Welsh, M. (2006). Theory of mind and executive function: Working-memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(3), 819–835.
- Nadig, A., Lee, I., Singh, L., Bosshart, K., & Ozonoff, S. (2010). How does the topic of conversation affect verbal exchange and eye gaze? A comparison between typical development and high-functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2730–2739. doi: 2710.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.2705.2020. Epub 2010 May 2721.
- Nadig, A., Vivanti, G., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: A comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Res., 2(6), 334–347. doi: 310.1002/aur.1102.
- Nadig, A.S., & Sedivy, J.C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13(4), 329–336.
- Naito, M., & Nagayama, K. (2004). Autistic children's use of semantic common sense and theory of mind: A comparison with typical and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 507–519.
- Narzisi, A., Muratori, F., Calderoni, S., Fabbro, F., & Urgesi, C. (2013). Neuropsychological profile in high functioning autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(8), 1895–1909. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0
- Nilsen, E.S., & Graham, S.A. (2009). The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 220–249.
- Olivar-Parra, J.S., De-La-Iglesia-Gutierrez, M., & Forns, M. (2011). Training referential communicative skills to individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. Psychological reports, 109(3), 921–939. doi: 10.2466/10.11.15.28.PR0.109.6.921-939
- Oswald, D.P., & Ollendick, T.H. (1989). Role taking and social competence in autism and mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(1), 119–127.
- Paul, R. (2007). Communication and its development in autism spectrum disorders. In F. Volkmar (Ed.), Autism and pervasive developmental disorders ( 2nd ed., pp. 129–155). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205–220.
- Paul, R., & Cohen, D.J. (1985). Comprehension of indirect requests in adults with autistic disorders and mental retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 475–479.
- Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530–544. doi: 10.1037/a0018287
- Rajendran, G., Mitchell, P., & Rickards, H. (2005). How do individuals with Asperger syndrome respond to nonliteral language and inappropriate requests in computer-mediated communication? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 429–443. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-5033-z
- Reed, T. (1994). Performance of autistic and control subjects on three cognitive perspective-taking tasks. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(1), 53–66.
- Reed, T. (2002). Visual perspective taking as a measure of working memory in participants with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 14(1), 63–76.
- Resches, M., & Perez Pereira, M. (2007). Referential communication abilities and Theory of Mind development in preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 34(1), 21–52.
- Roid, G. (2003). Stanford-Binet intelligence scales ( 5th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
- Rubin, R., Brown-Schmidt, S., Duff, M., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. (2009). Common ground representations in hippocampal amnesia. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience, Chicago, IL.
- Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
- Sanders, J., Johnson, K., Garavan, H., Gill, M., & Gallagher, L. (2008). A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autism spectrum disorders: Attention, inhibition and cogntive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(1), 1–16.
- Schuh, J.M., & Eigsti, I.M. (2012). Working memory, language skills, and autism symptomatology. Behavioral Sciences, 2, 207–218. doi: 10.3390/bs2040207
- Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W.A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals ( 4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.
- Sheslow, D., & Adam, W. (1990). Wide range assessment of memory and learning. Wilmington, DE: Jastak.
- Stone, W.L., & Caro-Martinez, L.M. (1990). Naturalistic observations of spontaneous communication in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(4), 437–453.
- Suh, J., Eigsti, I.M., Naigles, L.R., Barton, M., Kelley, E., & Fein, D. (2014). Narrative performance of Optimal Outcome children and adolescents with a history of an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1681–1694. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2042-9
- Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A re-examination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P.R. Zelazo (Eds.), Development in autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 173–193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Uchiyama, H., Seki, A., Kageyama, H., Saito, D.N., Koeda, T., Ohno, K., & Sadato, N. (2006). Neural substrates of sarcasm: A functional magnetic-resonance imaging study. Brain Research, 1124(1), 100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.09.088
- Volden, J., Mulcahy, R.F., & Holdgrafer, G. (1997). Pragmatic language disorder and perspective taking in autistic speakers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(2), 181–198.
- Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children ( 4th ed.).i San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
- Wetherby, A.M. (1986). Ontogeny of communicative functions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 295–316.