Early View e70071
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Open Access

Vegetable-Related Parenting Practices, Parenting Style and Preschoolers' Vegetable Consumption: Cross-Sectional Associations and the Moderating Role of Parenting Style

Jenna Rahkola

Jenna Rahkola

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Reetta Lehto

Reetta Lehto

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Henna Vepsäläinen

Henna Vepsäläinen

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Anna M. Abdollahi

Anna M. Abdollahi

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Josefine Björkqvist

Josefine Björkqvist

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Institute of Applied Health Sciences, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Emmi Tilli

Emmi Tilli

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Nithya Serasinghe

Nithya Serasinghe

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Jessica Gubbels

Jessica Gubbels

Department of Health Promotion, NUTRIM Institute of Nutrition and Translational Research in Metabolism, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

Search for more papers by this author
Alissa J. Burnett

Alissa J. Burnett

Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University, Burwood, Victoria, Australia

Search for more papers by this author
Ester van der Borgh-Sleddens

Ester van der Borgh-Sleddens

Mondriaan Mental Health Center, Heerlen, the Netherlands

Search for more papers by this author
Eva Roos

Eva Roos

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Department of food studies, Nutrition and Dietetics, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden

Search for more papers by this author
Maijaliisa Erkkola

Maijaliisa Erkkola

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Search for more papers by this author
Carola Ray

Corresponding Author

Carola Ray

Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland

Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Correspondence: Carola Ray ([email protected])

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 17 July 2025

ABSTRACT

The specific food parenting practices used by parents, along with the broader parenting style that reflects the general characteristics of their approach to parenting, may influence children's vegetable consumption. We examined cross-sectional associations of parenting style constructs (nurturance, structure, behavioural control, inappropriate control, and overprotection) and vegetable-related parenting practices (VPP) (‘encouragement and modelling’, ‘child involvement’, ‘enhanced availability and autonomy support’) with 3–6-year-old children's vegetable consumption. Additionally, we explored if parenting style moderated the associations between the VPPs and children's vegetable consumption. The sample included 767 Finnish children and their parents. Parents reported parenting style using an item-reduced Comprehensive General Parenting Questionnaire and the use of VPPs. Three-day food records were used to assess children's vegetable consumption. Data was analysed using linear mixed-effects models. If parents scored higher on ‘enhanced availability and autonomy support’, their children consumed more vegetables (B = 0.249, 95% CI = 0.128; 0.371). Regarding parenting style, if parents scored higher on overprotection, their children consumed less vegetables (B = −0.223, 95% CI = −0.384; −0.062). Additionally, two exploratory moderation effects by parenting style were found: parents' higher score on ‘child involvement’ was associated with children consuming more vegetables only when parents were lower on overprotection or nurturance. Although further research is needed to understand the details of the role of parenting style in children's vegetable consumption, our results suggest that overprotection might be an important factor, and parenting style as a context should be considered when targeting parenting practices to promote children's vegetable consumption.

Summary

  • Out of the vegetable-related parenting practices examined, parents using practices that enhance vegetable availability and accessibility at meals positively associated with preschoolers' vegetable consumption, which supports previous research.

  • Out of the parenting style constructs examined, overprotection inversely associated with preschoolers' vegetable consumption. This association is novel and requires further investigation.

  • As suggested by previous literature, parenting style may not have strong direct associations with children's vegetable consumption but may rather moderate associations between practices and children's vegetable consumption. This was partly supported by our findings. Interventions could benefit from considering parenting style as a contextual factor.

Abbreviations

  • CGPQ
  • Comprehensive General Parenting Questionnaire
  • DAGIS
  • Increased Health and Wellbeing in Children, Families and Educational Settings
  • ECEC
  • early childhood education and care
  • VPP
  • vegetable-related parenting practice
  • 1 Introduction

    Vegetable consumption below recommended levels among children has long been an issue in many countries, including Finland (Hamner et al. 2023; Koivuniemi et al. 2022; Kyttälä et al. 2010; Lynch et al. 2014). Promoting the consumption of vegetables in early childhood is an important part of establishing lasting healthy eating habits. Therefore, the determinants of children's vegetable consumption are an important area of research. Parents shape both the physical and psychosocial home food environment and thus have the power to influence what, how much, when, and how children eat (Niermann et al. 2018; Ventura and Birch 2008; Wardle and Cooke 2008).

    Constructs of parenting style and food parenting practices have been associated with preschool-aged children's (2–6 years) food consumption (Burnett et al. 2020; Yee et al. 2017). Research has identified numerous food parenting practices, which are specific actions parents implement within a food-related context, typically aimed at influencing children's eating (Kremers et al. 2013; Vaughn et al. 2016). Regarding preschool-aged children's vegetable consumption, controlling practices—such as pressure to eat—have been associated with lower consumption (Blissett 2011; Wardle et al. 2005). In contrast, practices like modelling healthy eating, making healthier foods available and accessible for the child at home, encouraging healthier intake, and involving the child in food preparation have been associated with higher consumption (Blissett 2011; Jarman et al. 2022; Kristiansen et al. 2017; Mazza et al. 2022; Yee et al. 2017). The evidence is more consistent for some practices than others, and most of the studies have been cross-sectional and relied on parent-report. Many of the practices associated with healthier food consumption are based on increasing exposure to healthier foods, and the effectiveness of repeated exposure is well-documented (Holley et al. 2017). Furthermore, food parenting practices targeted toward specific foods, such as vegetables (Baranowski et al. 2013), can be assessed. Parents may apply different practices depending on the food type, and certain practices might be more effective for promoting intake of some foods than others. However, it remains unclear whether examining food-specific practices provides added value compared to broader instruments that categorise foods into general groups such as ‘healthy’ and ‘unhealthy’ (Vaughn et al. 2016). There is a relative paucity of studies examining practices directed solely at vegetables and their association with children's vegetable consumption.

    Parenting style refers to a more global measure of the nature of parent behaviour across different situations reflecting the parent's attitudes and beliefs towards the child (Darling and Steinberg 1993). It can be seen as the larger emotional context within which (food) parenting practices occur and in which the parent's behaviours are interpreted by the child (Darling and Steinberg 1993). Parenting style is commonly assessed based on two dimensions (warmth/responsiveness and control/demandingness), the combination of which results in four styles: authoritative (high warmth and high control), authoritarian (low warmth and high control), permissive (high warmth and low control), and disengaged (low warmth and low control) (Baumrind 1971; Power 2013). While there are some inconsistencies, the authoritative parenting style has been most often associated with healthier dietary intake, including higher fruit and vegetable consumption, among preschool-aged children (Blissett 2011; Burnett et al. 2020).

    Recently, Sleddens et al. have developed a more detailed Comprehensive General Parenting Questionnaire (CGPQ) (Sleddens et al. 2014). It assesses five higher-order constructs of parenting (nurturance, structure, behavioural control, inappropriate control, and overprotection), and it was designed to capture different aspects of parenting—particularly forms of control—more extensively than previous instruments assessing parenting style (Sleddens et al. 2014). In short, nurturance refers to how responsive and supportive a parent is to their child's needs and autonomy. Structure describes the extent to which parents organise their child's environment in a consistent way to facilitate and foster child development, including setting boundaries. Behavioural control describes a form of control that is characterised by supervising and managing the child's activities in a neither over-controlling nor intrusive manner. Inappropriate control, in turn, is a form of control characterised by domination, intrusion, pressure and discouragement of child's autonomy. Overprotection, a third form of control, refers to excessive involvement or excessive nurturing that impedes the development of the child's autonomy. To our knowledge, only one study thus far has examined direct associations between parenting style assessed with the CGPQ and children's vegetable consumption. Among 288 children (6–12-year-olds) and their parents in Belgium, overprotection was correlated negatively with the child's vegetable consumption, although this association did not remain significant after adjusting for the child's age, child's gender, parental educational level, father's BMI, mother's BMI, child birth order and the other CGPQ constructs (Philips et al. 2014). Altogether, the associations between parenting style and children's vegetable consumption remain unclear, specifically regarding different forms of parental control.

    In addition to potentially having direct relationships with child food consumption, theoretical models suggest that parenting style may act as a moderator in associations between (food) parenting practices and the child's behaviour (e.g., food consumption) (Darling and Steinberg 1993; Niermann et al. 2018). Parenting style creates an emotional environment that partly determines how the child perceives the parenting practices (Darling and Steinberg 1993). Studies in both younger and older children suggest that these interactions may play a role in influencing children's food consumption (Blissett 2011; Yee et al. 2017). Overall, the findings have been rather mixed, and only a few studies have specifically examined the CGPQ constructs as moderators. For example, one Dutch study found that the CGPQ constructs moderated longitudinal associations between food parenting practices when the child was 6 years old and the child's snacking and consumption of sugar-sweetened drinks and fruits at age 8 (Sleddens et al. 2014).

    The objective of this study was two-fold: (1) to examine direct associations of the CGPQ constructs and three vegetable-related parenting practice (VPP) constructs (‘encouragement and modelling’, ‘child involvement’, ‘enhanced availability and autonomy support’) with children's vegetable consumption, and (2) to explore moderation by the CGPQ constructs in associations between the VPPs and children's vegetable consumption. Concerning the direct associations, we hypothesised that all VPP constructs would have a positive association with vegetable consumption, and, out of the CGPQ constructs, nurturance, structure, and behavioural control would have a positive association whereas overprotection and inappropriate control would have a negative association with vegetable consumption. To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the CGPQ constructs as potential moderators in the associations between the VPPs and vegetable consumption among young children.

    2 Methods

    2.1 Study Design and Participants

    The current study utilises cross-sectional survey data from a larger DAGIS research project in Finland (Määttä et al. 2015). The sample selection and data collection of the DAGIS survey have been described in detail previously (Lehto et al. 2018). Briefly, the DAGIS survey was conducted in eight municipalities from Western and Southern Finland in 2015–2016. Data were collected altogether through 66 public early childhood education and care (ECEC) centres. A total of 864 children aged 3–6 years and their legal guardians, hereafter referred to as parents, participated in the study (24% participation rate). The participants were from 760 families (i.e., distinct households), and there were 102 families with two or three participating children. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents. The DAGIS survey was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the University of Helsinki Ethical Review Board in the Humanities and Social and Behavioural Sciences deemed the study ethically acceptable in February 2015 (#6/2015).

    2.2 Children's Vegetable Consumption

    The outcome variable of children's vegetable consumption was computed based on a 3-day food record. Children's food consumption on 2 weekdays (outside preschool hours) and 1 weekend day was reported by parents. Early educators reported children's food consumption at the ECEC centres on the same 2 weekdays as for which the parents reported consumption outside preschool hours. The food record template for parents included written instructions and an example page. Early educators received a separate pre-coded food record accompanied by written and oral instructions. The parents and early educators both received a validated Children's Food Picture Book (Nissinen et al. 2015) that they could utilise when estimating portion sizes. Food records were reviewed by trained researchers who contacted the participants to obtain missing details in the reported foods with added emphasis on vegetable, fruit, and sugary product consumption [see (Korkalo et al. 2019) for more details about the food record data collection]. Trained researchers then computed the dietary intake with AivoDiet dietary software 2.2.0.0 (Mashie FoodTech Solutions Finland Oy), which utilised the Fineli Food Composition Database (Release 16, 2013) administered by the Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare. Only children with food record data from all 3 days were included in this study, which resulted in a sample of 767 children (89% of participants). In this study, fresh, frozen or canned vegetables (cooked or not) eaten as such (incl. salads containing mainly vegetables) were included in the variable of the child's vegetable consumption. Vegetables as ingredients in composite dishes were not included. Additionally, legumes (incl. pulses) were included, but potatoes were excluded from the variable. To reveal differences in vegetable consumption independently of energy intake (≈amount of food eaten), vegetable consumption was calculated as g/MJ for analyses. A mean for the 3 days was the final variable.

    2.3 Parenting Style

    A parent-reported 22-item version of the CGPQ was used to assess parenting style. The items are rated on a five-point Likert scale, from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). If more than one child from a household participated, this questionnaire was completed separately for each child. Though less comprehensive, the 22-item CGPQ still assesses all five higher-order constructs of parenting: nurturance, structure, behavioural control, inappropriate control and overprotection. This version was compiled based on responses to the original CGPQ for children aged 1–4 years (69 items) (van der Horst and Sleddens 2017) in a pilot study of the DAGIS project (2014) and responses to a 29-item version in the DAGIS survey (2015–2016) [see (Ray et al. 2022) for details of the item-reduction process]. The 22-item version demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency, good concordance with the 69-item version (intraclass correlation coefficients ranging from 0.51 to 0.94) and good convergent validity based on average variance extracted values over 0.5 (Ray et al. 2022). Furthermore, it met all the criteria of confirmatory factor analysis (Ray et al. 2022). Item means of the five constructs (see Supplementary Table S1 for scale information) comprised the parenting style variables used in this study as exposure and moderator variables. The mean was computed if there was information on all items constituting the construct. The Cronbach's alphas for the constructs ranged from 0.57 to 0.64 in the analytic sample of this study.

    2.4 Vegetable-Related Parenting Practices

    Twelve items adapted from a Habit scale of a Model of Goal-Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices instrument (Baranowski et al. 2013) were used to assess VPPs. One parent rated the items on a five-point Likert scale ranging from totally disagree (1) to totally agree (5). If more than one child from a household participated, these items were completed separately for each child. As the subscales form the original source could not be used, we used constructs that were formed in a previous study using the same data as in the current study (DAGIS survey): a principal component analysis resulted in three constructs: (1) encouragement and modelling (five items), (2) child involvement (three items) and (3) enhanced availability and autonomy support (four items) (Kaukonen et al. 2019). The first construct includes showing enjoyment of eating vegetables and verbally prompting the child to taste/eat vegetables. The second construct includes involving the child in choosing and preparing vegetables, and the third one describes offering opportunities to consume vegetables at meals while allowing the child to choose whether to eat or which ones to eat. An item mean was computed for these constructs if there was information on all items constituting a construct, and the means comprised the VPP variables examined as exposure variables in this study (see Supplementary Table S1 for construct information). The Cronbach's alphas ranged from 0.64 to 0.80 in the current analytic sample.

    2.5 Potential Confounding Factors

    The following child and family factors were considered as potential confounders in this study: the gender, age and BMI z-score of the child, the educational level of the family, the number of children in the household and whether the parent was a single parent. These were selected based on the confounders used in previous studies (Alsharairi and Somerset 2015; Burnett et al. 2019; O'Connor et al. 2010; Philips et al. 2014) and on likely associations with the exposures and the outcome (Jarman et al. 2022; Taraban and Shaw 2018; Vaughn et al. 2016). The child's date of birth and gender were reported by one parent. Child's height and weight were measured by trained researchers in the ECEC centres (Leppänen et al. 2019). The body mass index [weight (kg)/height2 (m)] was then calculated and converted into an age- and sex-specific BMI z-score according to Finnish growth references (Saari et al. 2011). Parents were also asked to report their and their spouse's educational level, number of children and adults living in the household and the living arrangements of the child (e.g., living together with both parents). A three-category variable indicating the highest education in the family (low = comprehensive, vocational or high school; middle = bachelor's degree or equivalent; and high = master's degree, licentiate or doctor) was computed. A variable indicating single parenthood was derived from the number of adults in the household and the living arrangements of the child. Thus, single parenthood was defined as a one-adult household where the child lived full-time. Furthermore, the respondent (mother, stepmother, father, stepfather or other caregiver) of the questionnaire assessing background factors, parenting style and VPPs will be presented to describe the study sample.

    2.6 Statistical Analyses

    All statistical analyses were conducted with statistical software R version 4.4.0 (R Core Team 2022). The basic analytic sample of this study consisted of all children with adequate food record data to compute the 3-day mean for vegetable consumption (N = 767). Descriptive statistics are reported as frequencies, median with first and third quartile for non-normally distributed variables, and as mean with standard deviation for normally distributed variables. Differences in study variables between participants with complete and missing data on the main variables (vegetable consumption, parenting style, and VPPs) were explored using t-test, Mann–Whitney U test, or Chi-squared test.

    Linear mixed effects models (Bates et al. 2015) were used to examine the associations between the parenting style variables and the VPP variables and the child's vegetable consumption. This model was chosen to account for household-level clustering. The analytic sample included 676 households, with 171 children from households where more than one child participated. A family ID variable was included as a random intercept in each model. Intraclass correlation coefficients ranging from 0.72 to 0.75 supported the model choice. Residual Q-Q plots indicated non-normality; therefore, the outcome variable—vegetable consumption—was square-root transformed. Each model included participants with complete data for the variables in that specific model (case-wise deletion).

    Direct associations of parenting style variables and VPP variables with children's vegetable consumption were examined with three different models. First, we tested crude models, where only one parenting style or VPP variable was included at a time as the explanatory variable. Next, to account for potential confounders, we added them stepwise as covariates. Model 2 was adjusted for the child's age and gender. Finally, in addition to age and gender, Model 3 was adjusted for all potential confounders. For parenting style, it included all other parenting style variables, and for VPPs, it accounted for the other two VPP variables along with all parenting style variables. To avoid overadjustment, model 3 was not adjusted for the VPPs when investigating parenting style as exposure since parenting style could influence the child's vegetable consumption through the use of VPPs (Niermann et al. 2018).

    Moderation by parenting style was explored by adding interaction terms to the linear mixed effects models. Each interaction was examined in a separate model. We tested both the crude interaction models and the models adjusted for the confounders: child's age, child's gender, child's BMI z-score, family's educational level, single parenthood and number of children in the household. If an interaction term was significant at the level p < 0.10, it was examined further using the “interactions” package in R (Long 2024): a figure was created to visualise the interaction, and conditional associations between the focal exposure and the outcome at different values of the moderator (16th, 50th and 84th percentiles) were displayed (Hayes 2022). Other than for the threshold for probing an interaction (Fleiss 1986; McClelland and Judd 1993), the significance level for all analyses was set at 0.05.

    2.7 Ethics Statement

    The DAGIS survey study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the University of Helsinki Ethical Review Board in the Humanities and Social and Behavioural Sciences deemed the study ethically acceptable in February 2015 (#6/2015). Informed written consents were received from the guardians of the participating children.

    3 Results

    3.1 Descriptives of the Study Sample

    Table 1 presents the descriptives of the study sample. The average age of the children was 4.8, and the distribution of gender was 48% girls and 52% boys. The majority of the families had two parents (91%), and the most common number of children was two (53%). Moreover, 78% of the families had a parent with at least a bachelor's degree or equivalent. In terms of parenting style, all constructs had a rather high median (≥ 4) relative to the scale maximum, with the exception of overprotection (median 2.60). Regarding the VPP constructs, encouragement and modelling also had a median over 4. When comparing participants with complete and missing data (data not shown), we found that participants who had missing data on either parenting style (p = 0.027) or vegetable consumption (p < 0.001) had a lower educational level.

    TABLE 1. Descriptives of the study sample (N = 767).
    N (%) Mean (SD) Median (Q1/Q3)
    Child's gender
    Girl 365 (48)
    Boy 402 (52)
    Questionnaire filled in by
    Mother 649 (85)
    Father 83 (11)
    Other 3 (0.3)
    Missing 32 (4)
    Highest education in the family
    Low 162 (21)
    Middle 324 (42)
    High 278 (36)
    Missing 3 (0.3)
    Single parenthood
    Yes 40 (5)
    No 698 (91)
    Missing 29 (4)
    Child age (years) 4.77 (0.9)
    Child BMI z-score −0.03 (1.0)
    Number of children in the household 2.00 (2.0/3.0)
    Daily vegetable intake (g/MJ) 11.3 (6.7/17.8)
    Daily vegetable intake (g) 65.33 (37.5/98.8)
    Parenting style (scale 1–5)
    Nurturance 4.14 (3.9/4.4)
    Structure 4.50 (4.0/4.8)
    Behavioural control 4.50 (4.0/4.8)
    Inappropriate control 4.00 (3.0/4.5)
    Overprotection 2.60 (2.2/3.2)
    VPPs (scale 1–5)
    Encouragement and modelling 4.40 (4.0/4.8)
    Child involvement 3.33 (2.7/4.0)
    Enhanced availability and autonomy support 3.50 (2.8/4.0)
    • Abbreviations: VPP, vegetable-related parenting practice; Q1, first quartile; Q3, third quartile.
    • a low = comprehensive, vocational or high school; middle = bachelor's degree or equivalent; and high = master's degree, licentiate or doctor.
    • b Finnish growth references (Saari et al. 2011).

    3.2 Direct Associations of VPPs and Parenting Style With Child Vegetable Consumption

    As can be seen in Table 2, the VPP construct of enhanced availability and autonomy support had a statistically significant positive association with the child's vegetable consumption—also after full adjustments (model 3: B = 0.249, 95% CI = 0.128; 0.371). Hence, if parents used more enhanced availability and autonomy support practices, their child consumed more vegetables. In the crude model, enhanced availability and autonomy support explained 2.5% of the variance in children's vegetable consumption (Pseudo-R2 = 0.025). To aid interpretation, we also report the result from the fully adjusted model using nonenergy-adjusted, non-transformed vegetable consumption: a one-unit increase in the enhanced availability and autonomy support score corresponded to an increase of 9.6 grams in vegetable consumption (B = 9.64, 95% CI = 4.73; 14.54). The other two VPP constructs (child involvement; encouragement and modelling) were not associated with children's vegetable consumption in any model.

    TABLE 2. Associations of vegetable-related parenting practice constructs and parenting style constructs with children's vegetable consumption (square-root transformed). Separate models for each exposure.
    Crude model, N = 728–734 Model 2, N = 728–734 Model 3, N = 672–675
    B 95% CI p-value B 95% CI p-value B 95% CI p-value
    VPPs
    Encouragement and modelling 0.061 −0.072; 0.193 0.365 0.067 −0.066; 0.200 0.318 0.056 −0.089; 0.201 0.448
    Child involvement 0.071 −0.019; 0.161 0.122 0.068 −0.023; 0.159 0.141 −0.009 −0.117; 0.098 0.868
    Enhanced availability and autonomy support 0.228 0.121; 0.335 < 0.001 0.228 0.120; 0.334 < 0.001 0.249 0.128; 0.371 < 0.001
    Parenting style
    Nurturance −0.091 −0.293; 0.112 0.379 −0.087 −0.290; 0.115 0.398 −0.105 −0.356; 0.146 0.417
    Structure 0.005 −0.193; 0.204 0.957 0.012 −0.189; 0.212 0.909 0.096 −0.156; 0.349 0.459
    Behavioural control −0.021 −0.188; 0.146 0.803 −0.020 −0.188; 0.147 0.812 0.060 −0.148; 0.269 0.573
    Inappropriate control −0.073 −0.180; 0.035 0.184 −0.073 −0.180; 0.034 0.181 −0.039 −0.162; 0.084 0.535
    Overprotection −0.180 −0.318; −0.042 0.011 −0.181 −0.320; −0.043 0.011 −0.223 −0.384; −0.062 0.007
    • Abbreviation: VPP, vegetable-related parenting practice.
    • Model 2 is adjusted for the child's age and gender.
    • Model 3 is additionally adjusted for the child's BMI z-score, family's educational level, number of children in the household, single parenthood and, when examining a VPP, for parenting style variables and the other VPPs or, when examining a parenting style variable, only for the other parenting style variables.
    • Significant p-values are bolded.

    Associations between parenting style constructs and children's vegetable consumption are also displayed in Table 2. Overprotection had a negative association with children's vegetable consumption—also after adjusting for all confounders (model 3: B = −0.223, 95% CI = −0.384; −0.062). In other words, overprotective parents had children with lower vegetable consumption. In the crude model, overprotection explained 0.9% of the variance in children's vegetable consumption (Pseudo-R2 = 0.009). Again, to aid interpretation, we report the result from the fully adjusted model using nonenergy-adjusted, non-transformed vegetable consumption: a one-unit increase in the overprotection score corresponded to a decrease of 7.4 g in vegetable consumption (B = −7.37, 95% CI = −13.87; −0.89). The other four constructs (nurturance, structure, behavioural control and inappropriate control) were not associated with children's vegetable consumption.

    3.3 Parenting Style as a Moderator

    The interaction terms for nurturance and overprotection with the VPP construct of child involvement were statistically significant in both the crude and the adjusted models (p < 0.10). Results for each tested interaction term between parenting style constructs and VPP constructs in association to children's vegetable consumption can be found in Supporting Information S2: Table S2. The significant interactions were probed using the adjusted models. As can be seen in Figure 1, at lower levels of nurturance, the positive association between child involvement and the child's vegetable consumption was stronger. The association was significant when nurturance was set at 3.71 (16th percentile) and at 4.14 (50th percentile), but the association was not significant when nurturance was set at 4.57 (84th percentile) (Table 3). Overprotection had a similar moderation effect to nurturance. Figure 2 visualises overprotection as a moderator; higher child involvement was more strongly associated with higher child vegetable consumption at lower levels of overprotection. The association was significant only when overprotection was set at 2.00 (16th percentile) but not when overprotection was set at 2.60 (50th percentile) or 3.40 (84th percentile) (Table 3).

    Details are in the caption following the image
    Moderation by nurturance in the association between child involvement and child vegetable consumption. A higher score in nurturance refers to a more nurturing parenting style, and a higher score in child involvement refers to greater use of involvement practices. Child vegetable consumption (g/MJ) was square-root transformed. Nurturance ranged from 1.7 to 5.
    TABLE 3. Conditional associations between the vegetable-related parenting practice construct of child involvement and the child's vegetable consumption at different values of nurturance and overprotection.
    Moderator Value B 95% CI p-value
    Nurturance 3.71 0.180 0.038; 0.322 0.013
    4.14 0.103 0.005; 0.202 0.041
    4.57 0.027 −0.096; 0.149 0.670
    Overprotection 2.00 0.160 0.024; 0.296 0.022
    2.60 0.090 −0.006; 0.186 0.066
    3.40 −0.003 −0.137; 0.131 0.965
    • Note: Nurturance ranged from 1.7 to 5, and overprotection ranged from 1 to 4.6.
    • a Child vegetable consumption (g/MJ) is a square-root-transformed variable.
    • b Value of the variable at the 16th, 50th and 84th percentiles.
    Details are in the caption following the image
    Moderation by overprotection in the association between child involvement and child vegetable consumption. A higher score in overprotection refers to a more overprotective parenting style, and a higher score in child involvement refers to greater use of involvement practices. Child vegetable consumption (g/MJ) was square-root transformed. Overprotection ranged from 1 to 4.6.

    4 Discussion

    In our sample of 3–6-year-olds, parents' greater use of practices pertaining to the enhanced availability and autonomy support construct was associated with the child consuming more vegetables. Moreover, a higher score on the parenting style construct of overprotection was associated with the child consuming less vegetables. Additionally, two exploratory moderation effects of parenting style were found. However, it is also worth noting that several of the associations studied were not statistically significant.

    Although the variety of instruments used to assess food parenting practices and parenting style makes cross-study comparisons challenging, our results seem partly in line with previous research and our hypotheses. Regarding the VPP constructs examined, the positive association between enhanced availability and autonomy support and vegetable consumption in this study aligns with the broader and rather consistent evidence on higher availability and accessibility of healthier foods at home being associated with children's healthier food consumption (Blissett 2011; Yee et al. 2017). For example, among 395 Norwegian 3–5-year-olds, higher availability and accessibility of vegetables at home were associated with higher frequency and amount of vegetables consumed by the child (Kristiansen et al. 2017). That study had similar accessibility items to our enhanced availability and autonomy support items. Moreover, parents making healthy foods available and unhealthy foods less accessible at home were associated with higher vegetable consumption among Norwegian 1–6-year-olds (Mazza et al. 2022). The potential positive impact of availability and accessibility have been attributed to the fact that children will mostly eat what is available to them (Yee et al. 2017) and that repeated exposure (including only sight) can increase the familiarity of and preference for food items (Holley et al. 2017; Jago et al. 2007). The items of the enhanced availability and autonomy support construct in the current study also included an aspect of allowing the child some autonomy in situations where vegetables are served to them. Research has shown it is likely beneficial for children to have some control, age appropriately, over their food consumption, especially regarding how much they eat (Lohse and Mitchell 2021; Vaughn et al. 2016). Our result seems to support this. However, the potential benefits of allowing children control over their food consumption may depend on the type and extent of control. For example, higher ‘child control’ assessed with the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire has been associated with lower vegetable consumption in some studies (e.g., Quah et al. 2018).

    Somewhat unexpectedly, the constructs of child involvement and encouragement and modelling, were not associated with children's vegetable consumption. Parents modelling healthy eating has been associated with healthier food consumption among children, including higher vegetable consumption, in several previous studies (Blissett 2011; Vaughn et al. 2016; Yee et al. 2017). Involving the child in planning and preparing meals and encouraging healthier consumption have also previously been associated with higher vegetable consumption in some studies involving young children (Blissett 2011; de Jong et al. 2015; Kristiansen et al. 2017; Vaughn et al. 2016). However, null results also exist (Mazza et al. 2022; Shim et al. 2016). In the current study, the distribution of the encouragement and modelling variable was quite narrow, skewed towards high scores, which may have reduced the ability to detect an association.

    In terms of parenting style, the overprotection construct was negatively associated with child vegetable consumption. This is in line with a negative correlation between overprotection and children's vegetable consumption found among 6–12-year-olds in Belgium (Philips et al. 2014). However, the association did not remain after adjusting for confounders in that study. In the short version of the CGPQ we used in the current study, overprotection consists of items pertaining to the subdimension of excessive involvement (example items: “I always help my child with everything he/she does”, and “I carefully plan my child's day so that he/she has enough activities to keep him/her busy”). Thus, an overprotective parent is highly involved with and supervises over the child, which discourages independent behaviour to the extent that the child's autonomy development can be impaired (Sleddens et al. 2014). As the authors of the Belgian study (Philips et al. 2014) also speculated, overprotective parenting might create a pressuring atmosphere at mealtimes, which could consequently lead to lower vegetable consumption. On the other hand, we hypothesise that overprotective parents may be more likely to cater to the child's food preferences, which often do not include many vegetables, to avoid upsetting the child. Furthermore, parenting style may influence children's food consumption through differential use of food parenting practices (van der Horst and Sleddens 2017).

    Nurturance, structure, behavioural control and inappropriate control had no associations with vegetable consumption. Nurturance, structure and behavioural control are comparable to aspects of an authoritative parenting style (a combination of high levels of warmth/responsiveness with high levels of control/demandingness) (van der Horst and Sleddens 2017), which has previously been associated with higher vegetable consumption in many studies among young children (Burnett et al. 2020). Thus, associations with vegetable consumption could have been expected for nurturance, structure and behavioural control, even though different instruments are used to assess the authoritative parenting style. However, as parenting style is considered a distal predictor of children's food consumption (Niermann et al. 2018), associations between parenting style and children's food consumption are expected to be very small, if they exist. Moreover, nurturance, structure, and behavioural control showed relatively low variability, as most parents scored high on these constructs.

    Regarding the moderation by parenting style, our result suggests that the potential positive impact of involving the child in choosing vegetables and preparing them may be dependent on the parenting style context. We found that greater use of the child involvement practices was associated with higher children's vegetable consumption only when parents scored lower on nurturance or overprotection. We are not aware of previous studies that have examined the interaction between parenting style and VPPs using the same instruments to assess them as we did. Hence, direct comparisons are not possible. In one previous study using the CGPQ, encouragement and covert control practices were positively associated with children's fruit consumption and negatively with children's snacking when overprotection was lower (Sleddens et al. 2014). Another previous study found that when nurturance was lower, higher availability of healthy foods and lower availability of unhealthy foods at home was associated with the child consuming fewer pastries and sweets (Gubbels et al. 2020). Many studies have not found moderation by parenting style in associations between food parenting practices and children's vegetable consumption (Gubbels et al. 2020; Langer et al. 2017; Zhang et al. 2019). Hence, at this point, the possible mechanisms behind the interactions found in the current study are unknown. It could be speculated that when a parent is highly overprotective, they may be controlling in situations where the involving practices are used (perhaps overly controlling how the child completes the food preparation tasks, for instance), which consequently could impede the potentially positive impacts. The moderation effect of nurturance might indicate that the child involvement practices are more important for children who have parents with less optimal levels of nurturance, although, in the current study, the level of nurturance where the association was no longer significant was still very high. These findings regarding moderation by parenting style should be interpreted cautiously, given that a more lenient alpha threshold was used for the interaction terms (p < 0.10).

    Overall, the literature on parenting and children's food consumption is complicated by a diverse set of methodologies in assessing parenting as well as food consumption. This makes it difficult to compare and pinpoint the sources of differing study results. Especially for assessing parenting, developing consensus on standardised methodologies would be beneficial for future research. Nevertheless, this study adds to the important evidence base for designing interventions to promote vegetable consumption among children as well as for healthcare professionals who work with children and their parents to give advice on parenting. It is not entirely clear how malleable parenting style is (Morales et al. 2024), and as parenting style is a more distal factor associated with children's food consumption, it may not be as effective target for interventions as food parenting practices. Still, it may be an important aspect to consider when designing interventions that target food parenting practices to increase children's vegetable consumption, especially given its potential moderating effects. Tailoring interventions based on the parenting style of a parent might be an effective strategy.

    However, future research is needed to clarify the findings of this study, particularly regarding parenting style, in terms of causality, mechanisms, and clinical relevance. Furthermore, since a single dimension/construct does not describe the overall parenting style of a parent, and food parenting practices are not used in isolation (Sleddens et al. 2014; Vaughn et al. 2016), future research could examine profiles of parenting style derived from the CGPQ and profiles of VPP use (e.g., cluster analyses) and how they are associated with children's vegetable consumption. On a broader level, parenting styles and food parenting practices considered ‘positive’ have been associated with overall healthier dietary patterns in children—for example, lower consumption of sugary foods and beverages, alongside higher vegetable consumption (Burnett et al. 2020; Ventura and Birch 2008; Yee et al. 2017). However, there are important nuances. For example, a specific practice may be associated with the consumption of healthier foods but show no association with the consumption of unhealthier foods (e.g., Gubbels et al. 2020). Hence, future research could explore how the CGPQ constructs and the VPPs relate to other key food groups—particularly to assess whether VPPs may influence broader dietary intake despite being targeted specifically at vegetables.

    This study has several strengths, including the use of the CGPQ, which comprehensively captures different aspects of parental control over the child. However, it has been rarely used thus far. Another strength of this study is examining vegetable consumption separately from fruit consumption. Generally, preference for vegetables tends to be lower than preference for fruits among children (especially younger children) (Cooke and Wardle 2005), and it seems that vegetable consumption may have at least partly different determinants than fruit consumption (Mazza et al. 2022). In addition, the actual amount of vegetables consumed was examined, not just frequency, and a 3-day food record was used, which minimises recall bias (J.-S. Shim et al. 2014). However, future studies could examine not only the quantity but also the variety of vegetable consumption as an outcome, as both dimensions are important for health (Rosell and Fadnes 2024). Among the strengths of this study is also the age group, which is considered an important age for building healthy eating habits (Hodder et al. 2024).

    This study also has limitations to consider. Our cross-sectional data can only demonstrate associations, not causal relationships. It is likely that the associations between parenting and children's food consumption are bidirectional (Vaughn et al. 2016). It should also be noted that no multiple testing adjustments were adopted. Not accounting for vegetables contained in composite dishes is another limitation. However, this is unlikely to have substantially influenced the ranking of individuals (Lentjes et al. 2014). Additionally, the VPPs examined primarily target discrete vegetable consumption rather than the vegetables incorporated as ingredients in composite dishes. Thus, the potential associations can be expected to be detectable in the chosen variables. Nevertheless, future studies could strengthen these findings by also including vegetables consumed as part of composite dishes. One important consideration is that vegetable consumption in the ECEC centres was included. It is not directly controlled by parents and is influenced by early educators' practices, for instance (Lehto et al. 2019). This may have diluted the associations observed in this study. Although adapted from a validated measure, the VPP constructs used in this study lack formal validation. Moreover, Cronbach's alpha was moderately low for some of the parenting style and VPP constructs, although Cronbach's alpha over 0.5 can be considered acceptable (Portney and Watkins 2000). Assessing parenting behaviours by direct observation could provide valuable complementary insights in future research. The sample of this study might be biased to some extent, as indicated by the relatively low participation rate, the overrepresentation of higher educated parents compared to the general population of the same age in Finland (Statistics Finland 2023), and the higher likelihood of missing data on parenting style variables and children's vegetable consumption among less educated participants. As higher socioeconomic status has been associated with better dietary quality (Jarman et al. 2022) and more favourable parenting (Ayoub and Bachir 2023), it is also plausible that, relative to non-participants, participants may have had higher vegetable consumption and more favourable parenting behaviours. Additionally, since the parenting variables and children's vegetable consumption (outside preschool hours) were parent-reported, the data may be subject to social desirability bias. These factors limit the generalisability of the findings and may have attenuated the associations. However, we do not believe they affected the direction of the associations. A further limitation of the sample is that the parents were predominantly mothers. In addition, since parenting is a culturally bound construct, these associations need to be investigated in culturally/ethnically diverse samples.

    5 Conclusions

    Our finding that children consumed more vegetables when parents made vegetables more available at meals, while allowing the child to choose among them and portion them, supports previous evidence on the importance of availability and accessibility at home for children's vegetable consumption. Additionally, a relatively novel finding was that if parents were more overprotective, their children consumed less vegetables. Furthermore, our results provide partial support for theoretical models suggesting that parenting style may moderate the association between food parenting practices and children's food consumption. Specifically, two exploratory moderation effects were observed: higher use of practices that involve the child in choosing and preparing vegetables was associated with greater vegetable consumption only when parents scored lower on nurturance or overprotection. More research to elucidate the moderation effects of parenting style is necessary, as findings published so far have been mixed. All things considered, in addition to food parenting practices, parenting style may be an important factor to consider in efforts to promote young children's vegetable consumption, especially as a contextual factor given its potential moderation effects.

    Author Contributions

    Eva Roos is the principal investigator for the DAGIS project. Eva Roos and Maijaliisa Erkkola have been responsible for the funding of the project and collected data. Carola Ray, Reetta Lehto, and Henna Vepsäläinen were involved in collecting the data and/or conducting data management. Jenna Rahkola and Carola Ray conceptualised the present study. Jenna Rahkola performed the statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. Eva Roos, Carola Ray, Maijaliisa Erkkola, Reetta Lehto, Alissa J. Burnett, Estervan der Borgh-Sleddens, Jessica Gubbels, Henna Vepsäläinen, Anna M. Abdollahi, Josefine Björkqvist, Emmi Tilli, and Nithya Serasinghe provided feedback and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    Acknowledgements

    The authors thank the preschools, the preschool personnel, and the families for their participation in the DAGIS study, and the research staff for data collection. The authors thank the collaborating partners of the DAGIS study for providing assistance in designing the DAGIS study. The DAGIS study and this study were funded by Folkhälsan Research Centre, University of Helsinki, The Ministry of Education and Culture in Finland, The Ministry of Social Affairs and Health, The Academy of Finland (Grants: 285439, 287288, 288038, 315816), The Juho Vainio Foundation, The Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation, The Finnish Cultural Foundation/South Ostrobothnia Regional Fund, The Päivikki and Sakari Sohlberg Foundation, Medicinska Understödsföreningen Liv och Hälsa, Finnish Foundation for Nutrition Research, and Finnish Food Research Foundation. The funding bodies were not involved and did not interfere with the study at any stage. Open access publishing facilitated by Helsingin yliopisto, as part of the Wiley - FinELib agreement.

      Conflicts of Interest

      The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

      Data Availability Statement

      Data will be made available upon reasonable request. Researchers interested in the data from this study may contact principal investigator Eva Roos, [email protected].

        The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.