1. Introduction
Alarmingly increasing prevalence of Alzheimer′s disease (AD) due to the aging population in developing countries, combined with lack of standardized and conclusive diagnostic procedures, make accurate diagnosis of Alzheimer′s disease, especially for its early stage also known as amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a major public health concern. While no current medical treatment exists to stop or reverse this disease, recent dementia-specific pharmacological advances can slow its progression, making early diagnosis all the more important. Behaviourally, both AD and MCI are traditionally diagnosed in relation to abnormalities in brain functions such as memory, cognition, perception, and language. Furthermore, the differentiation of probable AD from other dementing illnesses is generally obtained by excluding alternative causes for cognitive dysfunction. It is important therefore to determine whether AD and MCI can be characterized by functional deficits other than high-level abnormalities already described and whether, with further development, they are specific and sensitive enough to contribute to the search of early markers of the disease process.
In an attempt to facilitate the diagnosis of AD, several noninvasive biomarkers have been proposed, including event-related potentials (ERPs). ERPs are voltage changes time-locked to some physical or mental occurrence in the ongoing electrical brain activity (recorded as EEG). Depending on the type of sensory stimulus, the ERPs can be divided into somatosensory, visual, or auditory ERPs. This review concerns the auditory modality.
In auditory ERP studies, perhaps the most commonly used experimental approach is the active oddball paradigm. In this paradigm, typically two classes of stimuli are presented, one occurring frequently (standard) and the other occurring infrequently (target), and the subject is required to distinguish between the two stimuli and to respond to the stimuli that are predesignated as targets. Variations of this paradigm include the passive oddball paradigm, in which the subject is instructed to ignore the stimuli, and so-called novelty oddball paradigm, in which a third class of stimuli (novelty) are also presented intermixed with the standard and target stimuli.
ERPs offer a psychophysiological method for studying attentional processes, language, and memory functions, yielding information not available from behavioral studies. A number of studies have suggested that ERPs are useful indices for assessing changes in cognitive brain functions. In particular, the P300 component of the ERP has been widely applied in the scientific study of age-related cognitive dysfunction, because it reflects attentional and memory processes. This ERP is most commonly elicited in a active oddball paradigm when a subject detects an occasional target stimulus in a regular train of standard stimuli. In the novelty oddball paradigm, in turn, deviant or unexpected tones elicit a frontal subcomponent of P300, namely, the P3a, which is considered as an electrophysiological marker of the orienting response [1]. Furthermore, in the passive oddball paradigm, at around 200 ms the deviant tones elicit a component called mismatch negativity (MMN). The MMN is thought to reflect the mismatch between a trace in a sensory memory (of the standard stimulus) and the representation of the current stimulus to which the trace is compared, and is considered to be an index of the preattentive stage of auditory information processing [2].
The present paper briefly reviews from the literature (especially from [3]) the background of clinical MMN and P300 applications.
2. P300 Responses
Auditory P300, a positive deflection occurring at about 300 ms from stimulus onset, is one of the most widely studied components of the ERP. It is generated by the activation of multiple neocortical and limbic regions, and has two functionally different components: the earlier P3a that is maximal over frontocentral regions, and the later P3b (hereafter called P300 in this review) that is maximal at posterior scalp locations [4].
3. Psychophysiology of P300
The P300 is parietocentral positivity that occurs when a subject detects an informative task relevant stimulus (first described by Desmedt et al. [5]; Sutton et al. [6]). It is most commonly elicited in an active oddball paradigm when a subject detects an occasional target stimulus in a regular train of standard stimuli. The P300 probably represent concurrent activity in multiple regions of the brain, including temporol-parietal neocortical areas and higher limbic structures [7–16].
The major theoretical interpretation of the P300 component is that it indexes updating of activity in corticolimbic circuits in processes requiring attention and working memory [17, 18]. This context updating theory has its roots in Sokolov’s model of the orienting response, which has been postulated to result from a change in the organism’s neural representation of the stimulus [19]. P300 amplitude is also proportional to the amount of attentional resources devoted to a given task [20–22] and has been associated with superior memory performance [23, 24]. P300 amplitude can therefore be viewed as a measure of CNS activity that reflects the processing of incoming information when it is incorporated into memory representations of the stimulus and the context in which the stimulus occurs. Variation in P300 amplitude is, therefore, assumed to reflect the degree or quality with which that information is processed.
The P300 has a latency to peak of anywhere from 300 to 1000 ms, depending on task complexity and the clinical sample tested. A frequently observed phenomenon is that the P300 latency increases when categorization of the stimulus becomes more difficult. A general consensus seems to be that P300 is evoked after the stimulus has been evaluated [25]. Thus, the latency of P300 has been regarded as a measure of stimulus evaluation time [26, 27]) and is generally unrelated to response selection processes [28, 29]. It is therefore independent of behavioral reaction time [30, 31]. Indeed, it is just these properties that make the P300 a valuable tool for assessing cognitive function: because P300 latency is an index of the processing time required before response generation, it is a sensitive temporal measure of the neural activity underlying the processes of attention allocation and immediate memory. In addition, P300 latency is negatively correlated with mental functions in normal subjects, with shorter latencies associated with superior cognitive performance (e.g., [32–35]). The neuropsychological tests that are best correlated with P300 latency are those that assess how rapidly subjects can allocate and maintain attentional resources. This association is also supported by results indicating that P300 latency increases as cognitive capability decreases from dementing illness [27, 35–40]. Thus, P300 latency is directly associated with cognitive capability in both normal and patient populations.
4. Clinical Applications of P300
Changes in the latency, amplitude, and topography of the P300 correlate with clinical findings in a wide range of disorders and brain injuries. Since the P300 has been related to the fundamental cognitive events of stimulus evaluation and immediate memory in normals, and because its peak latency is correlated with neuropsychological tests of cognitive function, this ERP component may provide an objective index of the degree of dementing illness which can be distinguished from the electrophysiological changes found in normal aging. Indeed, the initial suggestion that the P300 component might be a useful tool for investigating cognitive functions came from studies of normal aging and dementia, since peak latency was found to be prolonged in individuals with dementing illness compared to similarly aged normal subjects [41, 42]. The extent of deviation varied with the aetiology of the disorder, being greatest with metabolic causes and brain tumours and least with degenerative disorders, such as AD [41]. The P300 latency changes were reversed by treatment in patients with metabolic encephalopathy, with latency returning to normal values when the disorder was corrected and cognitive functions were again normal [43, 44].
Several studies have now verified that P300 is an objective and sensitive tool for demonstrating cognitive impairment in AD, as these patients have increased P300 latency and decreased P300 amplitude compared to elderly controls subjects [35, 45, 46]. P300 is sensitive to AD processes already during its early stages [47], and similar P300 alterations have also been demonstrated in MCI [48–50]. P300 amplitude or latency alterations may also identify preclinical changes in participants who are at relatively high risk for AD because of genetic predisposition [48, 51]. P300 may thus reveal neurophysiological changes prior to the emergence of clinical deficits, which could advance the early detection and diagnosis of AD.
P300 latency increases systematically as cognitive function becomes worse in dementing illness, even though component size is not directly associated with the degree of mental impairment [27, 40, 52]. Recently, in a followup study, it was shown that the abnormalities in P300 in AD and MCI latency correlated with the severity of cognitive impairment. Furthermore, upon followup, one year later after the baseline study, the P300 latencies demonstrated significantly more prolongation than their baseline measures in AD and MCI patients, although their neurophysiological evaluation showed no statistical decline, suggesting that the P300 latency may reflect cognitive decline more sensitively than neuropsychological tests in the longitudinal followup of AD patients [53]. It has also been suggested that P300 latency is a valuable tool for the evaluation of cholinesterase inhibitors treatment in demented patients [54]. However, P300 latency does not seem to be capable of predicting which MCI patients will convert to AD [48], and therefore seems to have no predictive value for AD diagnosis.
Some reports have suggested that ERP measures may distinguish between subcortical (e.g., Huntington’s and Parkinson’s disease) and cortical (Alzheimer’s, cerebral vascular accident) dementias [55, 56]. Other studies have indicated that P300 latency can separate individuals with dementia from those with depression-associated pseudodementia [37, 57].
Associations between P300 latency and the level of cognitive function also have been reported in neurological disorders, in confusional states, and for posttraumatic syndromes (cf. [34, 36, 38, 43, 44, 58–61]). Furthermore, the P300 component has been used to study psychiatric disorders such as alcoholism, depression, and schizophrenia (e.g., [62–67]). Taken together, these findings suggest that P300 may be clinically useful as an index of cognitive function, although its diagnostic utility is questionable (cf. [27, 37, 40, 68]). The P300 continues to be an important signature of cognitive processes such as attention and working memory and of its dysfunction in neurologic and mental disorders [69].
5. Psychophysiology of P3a
The P3a is a frontocentrally maximal positive ERP wave elicited by deviant or unexpected events [4, 70], and it is considered as an electrophysiological marker of the attentional switching, that is, the orienting response [1]. P3a is generated by a complex cerebral network, including the prefrontal, cingulate, temporo-parietal, and hippocampal regions [7, 71–74] and it is recorded over widespread anterior and posterior scalp sites [73]. It has been distinguished from P300 by a shorter peak latency, a more frontally oriented scalp topography and different elicitation conditions [4].
6. Clinical Applications of P3a
The P3a is affected in several psychiatric and neurological disorders. An enhanced P3a amplitude over the left frontal region has been found in chronic alcoholism [75]. An enhanced P3a are found in children with depression [76] and ADHD [77]. In addition, patients with closed head injuries show larger P3a amplitudes than control subjects [78, 79].
There are only a few studies published about P3a in AD and the findings have been to some extent inconsistent. Some authors found that AD patients are characterized by longer P3a latency than control subjects suggesting delayed orientation to deviant stimuli in AD [49, 80]. Furthermore, these authors suggested that separation of P3 subcomponents (P3a and P300) by dipole source analysis may increase sensitivity and specificity in correctly detecting AD patients from healthy subjects [49, 80]. On the other hand, some authors found no difference in the P3a between AD patients and controls but instead showed that the P3a was different in AD patients compared with patients with vascular dementia whereas the P300 was similar in these patients [81].
7. Psychophysiology of MMN
The mismatch negativity (MMN) is a frontal negativity at around 100–200 ms. It is generated automatically whenever the stimulus deviates physically from the immediately preceding context [82, 83]. MMN can be elicited by changes in simple tones, such as frequency or duration, and also by complex sounds such as phonemes [2]. The MMN is commonly derived by subtracting the ERP to the standard stimulus from that to the deviant stimulus. The MMN is thought to reflect the mismatch between a trace in a sensory memory (of the standard stimulus) and the representation of the current stimulus to which the trace is compared and is considered to be an index of the preattentive stage of auditory information processing [2]. In addition, by measuring the decay of the MMN amplitude as a function of the interstimulus interval, it is possible to estimate the duration of sensory memory. The MMN is generated mainly in the auditory cortex in the temporal lobes [84, 85]. Furthermore, a frontal MMN generator [86], has also been implicated.
8. Clinical Applications of MMN
MMN is an important ERP measure as it may reveal deficits of both sensory memory storage and of fundamental automatic mismatch detection mechanism [87, 88]. MMN is attention independent and therefore particularly suitable for studies with subjects who do not cooperate at all or cooperate very poorly. Clinical research lines using the MMN involve schizophrenia, dyslexia, autism spectrum disorders, coma, alcoholism, and dementia (for a review, see [89]).
Early studies on the aging effects on the MMN show that the MMN is smaller and prolonged in the ERPs of the normal old compared to those of the young (e.g., [90, 91]). In subsequent studies, Pekkonen et al. [92, 93] found that with frequency changes this effect was confined to conditions with long inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs), indicating that it is sensory memory rather than perception that is affected by aging. Similarly, several studies have found fairly unaffected MMN in AD at short ISIs [94]; (for review, see [95]), whereas MMN was reduced at long (3s) ISI in these patients [96]. These results suggest that AD appears to reduce the duration of auditory sensory memory when sound frequency is involved.
Interestingly, the pattern with duration MMN is quite different, with the age-related MMN-amplitude decrement being present even with short ISIs (for a review, see [95]). However, in patients with AD automatic stimulus discrimination to duration change in the auditory cortex is preserved as compared with normal aging [97]. These findings imply that although the preattentive discrimination to duration deviance is attenuated in aging, it is not further damaged in the early phase of AD. This may be due to the fact that the neurodegenerative changes underlying AD mainly affect mesial temporal structures like hippocampus, whereas the lateral aspects of the temporal lobes, where the MMN is generated, are less damaged [97].
In summary, studies on MMN in AD demonstrate that older controls and patients with AD produce MMNs that are reduced in amplitude relative to the younger subjects, but the differences between older controls and AD subjects are relatively small. Also, the fact that the AD subjects can produce significant MMN responses suggest that they do have a relatively intact MMN, albeit reduced in amplitude compared to controls [94]. In all the aforementioned studies, patients had minor to moderate cognitive impairment and taking into account the acknowledged cholinergic hypothesis in AD, probably their cholinergic defect was not sufficient to cause MMN generator impairment per se at short ISIs, but in some studies, impaired the duration of the memory trace [96].
As reviewed here, the value of the MMN in the early diagnosis of AD is somewhat limited. However, more pronounced MMN alterations have been found in demented Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients relative to normal controls or patients with AD and dementia with Lewy-bodies, indicating that demented PD patients to a larger degree than the control groups have a deficit in automatic auditory change detection [98]. Furthermore, MMN may aid in the differentiation of normal pressure hydocephalus (NPH) from NPH with concomitant AD [99]. Thus, the MMN may aid in the differentiation of AD from other dementing illnesses. These findings also have implications for understanding cognitive and behavioural functioning in patients with dementia.
9. Conclusion
Alzheimer’s disease is a neurodegenerative disorder, causing neuronal death that leads to cognitive function decline. Two misfolded proteins, β-amyloid that causes plagues and hyperphosphorylated-τ that causes neurofibrillary tangles are often blamed, yet, the genesis of these proteins, and in fact the true cause of the disease, are still unknown. While no current medical treatment exists to stop or reverse this disease, recent dementia-specific pharmacological advances can slow its progression, making early diagnosis all the more important.
Application of the auditory ERPS to the study of dementing illness and AD has produced positive findings. The P300 response, in particular, has become popular in studies of dementia. Because the P300 response is related to fundamental aspects of cognitive function in normals, it should be useful in the diagnosis of dementia, especially that of the Alzheimer’s type. In general, this assertion is supported by a wide variety of previous findings that include the spectrum of dementias. Although the P300 does not appear to differentiate between types of cortical dementias, it does accurately reflect the level of cognitive dysfunction caused by these disorders. Furthermore, the auditory evoked potentials (including the MMN) might offer an additional tool to index cholinergic dysfunction in aging and in neurodegenerative diseases such as AD. Moreover, when variables which affect P3 measures such as task parameters and population differences are well controlled, the P3 ERP can differentiate between early AD patients and normal controls. Given these effects, it is reasonable to suppose that further refinement of the test procedures would facilitate the delineation of differences in the P3 response for the early diagnosis of AD.
- 1
Squires K. C.,
Squires N. K., and
Hillyard S. A., Decision-related cortical potentials during an auditory signal detection task with cued observation intervals, Journal of Experimental Psychology. (1975) 1, no. 3, 268–279, 2-s2.0-0016537187, https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.1.3.268.
- 2
Näätänen R., Attention and Brain Function, 1992, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, USA.
- 3
Polich J. and
Herbst K. L., P300 as a clinical assay: rationale, evaluation, and findings, International Journal of Psychophysiology. (2000) 38, no. 1, 3–19, 2-s2.0-0033801879, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00127-6.
- 4
Squires N. K.,
Squires K. C., and
Hillyard S. A., Two varieties of long latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1975) 38, no. 4, 387–401, 2-s2.0-0016653813, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(75)90263-1.
- 5
Desmedt J. E.,
Debecker J., and
Manil J., Demonstration of a cerebral electric sign associated with the detection by the subject of a tactile sensorial stimulus. The analysis of cerebral evoked potentials derived from the scalp with the aid of numerical ordinates, Bulletin de l′Academie Royale de Medecine de Belgique. (1965) 5, no. 11, 887–936, 2-s2.0-0013828589.
- 6
Sutton S.,
Braren M.,
Zubin J., and
John E. R., Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty, Science. (1965) 150, no. 3700, 1187–1188, 2-s2.0-0013854584.
- 7
Halgren E.,
Baudena P.,
Clarke J. M.,
Heit G.,
Liegeois C.,
Chauvel P., and
Musolino A., Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual stimuli. I. Superior temporal plane and parietal lobe, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1995) 94, no. 3, 191–220, 2-s2.0-0028966416, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(94)00259-N.
- 8
Halgren E.,
Baudena P.,
Clarke J. M.,
Heit G.,
Marinkovic K.,
Devaux B.,
Vignal J. P., and
Biraben A., Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1995) 94, no. 4, 229–250, 2-s2.0-0028913512, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(95)98475-N.
- 9
Halgren E.,
Squires N. K., and
Wilson C. L., Endogenous potentials generated in the human hippocampal formation and amygdala by infrequent events, Science. (1980) 210, no. 4471, 803–805.
- 10
JohnsonR.Jr., Auditory and visual P300s in temporal lobectomy patients: evidence for modality-dependent generators, Psychophysiology. (1989) 26, no. 6, 633–650, 2-s2.0-0024797750.
- 11
Verleger R.,
Heide W.,
Butt C., and
Kompf D., Reduction of P3b in patients with temporo-parietal lesions, Cognitive Brain Research. (1994) 2, no. 2, 103–116, 2-s2.0-0028168709.
- 12
Horn H.,
Syed N.,
Lanfermann H.,
Maurer K., and
Dierks T., Cerebral networks linked to the event-related potential P300, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. (2003) 253, no. 3, 154–159, 2-s2.0-0041328459, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-003-0419-4.
- 13
Mccarthy G.,
Luby M.,
Gore J., and
Goldman-Rakic P., Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI, Journal of Neurophysiology. (1997) 77, no. 3, 1630–1634, 2-s2.0-0031004314.
- 14
Menon V.,
Ford J. M.,
Lim K. O.,
Glover G. H., and
Pfefferbaum A., Combined event-related fMRI and EEG evidence for temporal-parietal cortex activation during target detection, NeuroReport. (1997) 8, no. 14, 3029–3037, 2-s2.0-0030863055.
- 15
Opitz B.,
Mecklinger A.,
Von Cramon D. Y., and
Kruggel F., Combining electrophysiological and hemodynamic measures of the auditory oddball, Psychophysiology. (1999) 36, no. 1, 142–147, 2-s2.0-0032957417, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577299980848.
- 16
Stevens A. A.,
Skudlarski P.,
Gatenby J. C., and
Gore J. C., Event-related fMRI of auditory and visual oddball tasks, Magnetic Resonance Imaging. (2000) 18, no. 5, 495–502, 2-s2.0-0033920839, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0730-725X(00)00128-4.
- 17
Donchin E., Presidential Address, 1980: surprise!…surprise?, Psychophysiology. (1981) 18, no. 5, 493–513.
- 18
Donchin M. C. E., Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?, Behavioral and Brain Sciences. (1988) 357–374.
- 19
Polich J., Habituation of P300 from auditory stimuli, Psychophysiology. (1989) 17, 19–28.
- 20
Wickens C.,
Kramer A.,
Vanasse L., and
Donchin E., Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources, Science. (1983) 221, no. 4615, 1080–1082.
- 21
Kramer A. F. and
Strayer D. L., Assessing the development of automatic processing: an application of dual-task and event-related brain potential methodologies, Biological Psychology. (1988) 26, no. 1–3, 231–267, 2-s2.0-0024264582, https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(88)90022-1.
- 22
Gonsalvez C. J. and
Polich J., P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval, Psychophysiology. (2002) 39, no. 3, 388–396, 2-s2.0-0036254427, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201393137.
- 23
Johnson R., F. Boller and J. Grafman, Event-related potential insights into the neurobiology of memory systems, Handbook of Neuropsychology, 1995, 10, Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 135–163.
- 24
Fabiani M.,
Karis D., and
Donchin E., Effects of mnemonic strategy manipulation in a Von Restorff paradigm, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1990) 75, no. 2, 22–35, 2-s2.0-0025189713.
- 25
Kok A., On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity, Psychophysiology. (2001) 38, no. 3, 557–577, 2-s2.0-0035022747, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201990559.
- 26
Kutas M.,
McCarthy G., and
Donchin E., Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time, Science. (1977) 197, no. 4305, 792–795, 2-s2.0-0017647295.
- 27
Polich J., Attention, probability, and task demands as determinants of P300 latency from auditory stimuli, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1986) 63, no. 3, 251–259, 2-s2.0-0022483569.
- 28
McCarthy G. and
Donchin E., A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time, Science. (1981) 211, no. 4477, 77–80, 2-s2.0-0019496660.
- 29
Pfefferbaum A.,
Christensen C.,
Ford J. M., and
Kopell B. S., Apparent response incompatibility effects on P3 latency depend on the task, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1986) 64, no. 5, 424–437, 2-s2.0-0022469617.
- 30
Duncan-Johnson C. C., P300 latency: a new metric of information processing, Psychophysiology. (1981) 18, no. 3, 207–215, 2-s2.0-0019561156.
- 31
Verleger R., On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry, Psychophysiology. (1997) 34, no. 2, 131–156, 2-s2.0-0030999978, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02125.x.
- 32
Polich J.,
Howard L., and
Starr A., P300 latency correlates with digit span, Psychophysiology. (1983) 20, no. 6, 665–669, 2-s2.0-0021021901.
- 33
Emmerson R. Y.,
Dustman R. E.,
Shearer D. E., and
Turner C. W., P3 latency and symbol digit performance correlations in aging, Experimental Aging Research. (1989) 15, no. 3-4, 151–159, 2-s2.0-0024954739.
- 34
Polich J. and
Martin S., P300, cognitive capability, and personality: a correlational study of university undergraduates, Personality and Individual Differences. (1992) 13, no. 5, 533–543, 2-s2.0-44049119401.
- 35
Polich J.,
Ladish C., and
Burns T., Normal variation of P300 in children: age, memory span, and head size, International Journal of Psychophysiology. (1990) 9, no. 3, 237–248, 2-s2.0-0025133604, https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(90)90056-J.
- 36
Squires N. K.,
Galbraith G., and
Aine C., D. Lehmann and E. Callaway, Event-related potential assessment of sensory and cognitive deficits in the mentally retarded, Human Evoked Potentials: Applications and Problems, 1979, Plenum Press, New York, NY, USA, 397–413.
- 37
Brown W. S.,
Marsh J. T., and
LaRue A., Event-related potentials in psychiatry: differentiating depression and dementia in the elderly, Bulletin of the Los Angeles neurological societies. (1982) 47, 91–107, 2-s2.0-0020437156.
- 38
Homberg V.,
Hefter H., and
Granseyer G., Event-related potentials in patients with Huntington′s disease and relatives at risk in relation to detailed psychometry, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1986) 63, no. 6, 552–569.
- 39
O′Donnell B. F.,
Friedman S.,
Swearer J. M., and
Drachman D. A., Active and passive P3 latency and psychometric performance: influence of age and individual differences, International Journal of Psychophysiology. (1992) 12, no. 2, 187–195, 2-s2.0-0026506951, https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(92)90010-9.
- 40
Polich J., Normal variation of P300 from auditory stimuli, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1986) 65, no. 3, 236–240, 2-s2.0-0022644447.
- 41
Goodin D. S.,
Squires K. C., and
Starr A., Long latency event-related components of the auditory evoked potential in dementia, Brain. (1978) 101, no. 4, 635–648, 2-s2.0-0018122006.
- 42
Goodin D. S.,
Squires K. C.,
Henderson B. H., and
Starr A., Age-related variations in evoked potentials to auditory stimuli in normal human subjects, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1978) 44, no. 4, 447–458, 2-s2.0-0018130896, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(78)90029-9.
- 43
Goodin D. S.,
Starr A.,
Chippendale T., and
Squires K. C., Sequential changes in the P3 component of the auditory evoked potential in confusional states and dementing illnesses, Neurology. (1983) 33, no. 9, 1215–1218, 2-s2.0-0020572667.
- 44
Goodin D. S.,
Squires K. C., and
Starr A., Variations in early and late event-related components of the auditory evoked potential with task difficulty, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1983) 55, no. 6, 680–686, 2-s2.0-0020527786, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90278-X.
- 45
Holt L. E.,
Raine A.,
Pa G.,
Schneider L. S.,
Henderson V. W., and
Pollock V. E., P300 topography in Alzheimer′s disease, Psychophysiology. (1995) 32, no. 3, 257–265, 2-s2.0-0028968955.
- 46
Polich J. and
Pitzer A., G. Comi, C. H. Lucking, J. Kimura, and R. M. Rossini, P300 in early Alzheimer’s disease: oddball task difficulty and modality effects, Clinical Neurophysiology: From Receptors to Perception, EEG Supplement, 1999, Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 281–287.
- 47
Polich J. and
Corey-Bloom J., Alzheimer′s disease and P300: review and evaluation of task and modality, Current Alzheimer Research. (2005) 2, no. 5, 515–525, 2-s2.0-28244449105.
- 48
Golob E. J.,
Irimajiri R., and
Starr A., Auditory cortical activity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: relationship to subtype and conversion to dementia, Brain. (2007) 130, no. 3, 740–752, 2-s2.0-33947180483, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl375.
- 49
Frodl T.,
Hampel H.,
Juckel G.,
Bürger K.,
Padberg F.,
Engel R. R.,
Möller H. J., and
Hegerl U., Value of event-related P300 subcomponents in the clinical diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer′s disease, Psychophysiology. (2002) 39, no. 2, 175–181, 2-s2.0-0036201076, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577202010260.
- 50
Golob E. J.,
Johnson J. K., and
Starr A., Auditory event-related potentials during target detection are abnormal in mild cognitive impairment, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2002) 113, no. 1, 151–161, 2-s2.0-0036150913, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00713-1.
- 51
Ally B. A.,
Jones G. E.,
Cole J. A., and
Budson A. E., The P300 component in patients with Alzheimer′s disease and their biological children, Biological Psychology. (2006) 72, no. 2, 180–187, 2-s2.0-33645001561, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.10.004.
- 52
Ball S. S.,
Marsh J. T.,
Schubarth G.,
Brown W. S., and
Strandburg R., Longitudinal P300 latency changes in Alzheimer′s disease, Journals of Gerontology. (1989) 44, no. 6, M195–M200, 2-s2.0-0024456241.
- 53
Lai C. L.,
Lin R. T.,
Liou L. M., and
Liu C. K., The role of event-related potentials in cognitive decline in Alzheimer′s disease, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2010) 121, no. 2, 194–199, 2-s2.0-75149123118, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2009.11.001.
- 54
Werber E. A., [email protected], Gandelman-Marton R.,
Klein C., and
Rabey J. M., The clinical use of P300 event related potentials for the evaluation of cholinesterase inhibitors treatment in demented patients, Journal of Neural Transmission. (2003) 110, no. 6, 659–669, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-003-0817-9.
- 55
Rosenberg C.,
Nudleman K., and
Starr A., Cognitive evoked patentials (P300) in early Huntington′s disease, Archives of Neurology. (1985) 42, no. 10, 984–987, 2-s2.0-0022270550.
- 56
Goodin D. S. and
Aminoff M. J., Electrophysiological differences between subtypes of dementia, Brain. (1986) 109, no. 6, 1103–1113, 2-s2.0-0023036990.
- 57
Patterson J. V.,
Michalewski H. J., and
Starr A., Latency variability of the components of auditory event-related potentials to infrequent stimuli in aging, Alzheimer-type dementia, and depression, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1988) 71, no. 6, 450–460, 2-s2.0-0023732269.
- 58
O′Donnell B. F. and
Squires N. K., Evoked potential changes and neuropsychological performance in Parkinson′s disease, Biological Psychology. (1987) 24, no. 1, 23–37, 2-s2.0-0023102254.
- 59
Hansch E. C.,
Syndulko K., and
Cohen S. N., Cognition in Parkinson disease: an event-related potential perspective, Annals of Neurology. (1982) 11, no. 6, 599–607.
- 60
O′Donnell B. F.,
Friedman S.,
Squires N. K.,
Maloon A.,
Drachman D. A., and
Swearer J. M., Active and passive P3 latency in dementia. Relationship to psychometric, electroencephalographic, and computed tomographic measures, Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology. (1990) 3, no. 3, 164–179, 2-s2.0-0025145958.
- 61
Newton M. R.,
Barrett G.,
Callanan M. M., and
Towell A. D., Cognitive event-related potentials in multiple sclerosis, Brain. (1989) 112, no. 6, 1637–1660, 2-s2.0-0024846064.
- 62
Pritchard W. S., Cognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia, Psychological Bulletin. (1986) 100, no. 1, 43–66, 2-s2.0-0022749849, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.43.
- 63
Courchesne E., J. Rohrbaugh, R. Parasuraman, and R. Johnson, Chronology of postnatal human brain development: event-related potential, positron tomography, myelinogenesis, and synaptogenesis, Event-Related Brain Potentials: Basic Issues and Applications, 1990, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 210–241.
- 64
McCarley R. W.,
Shenton M. E.,
O′Donnell B. F.,
Faux S. F.,
Kikinis R.,
Nestor P. G., and
Jolesz F. A., Auditory P300 abnormalities and left posterior superior temporal gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia, Archives of General Psychiatry. (1993) 50, no. 3, 190–197, 2-s2.0-0027407078.
- 65
Begleiter H. and
Porjesz B., H. Begleiter and B. Kissin, Neurophysiological phenotypic factors in the development of alcoholism, The Genetics of Alcoholism, 1995, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 269–293.
- 66
Bruder G. E.,
Tenke C. E.,
Stewart J. W.,
Towey J. P.,
Leite P.,
Voglmaier M., and
Quitkin F. M., Brain event-related potentials to complex tones in depressed patients: relations to perceptual asymmetry and clinical features, Psychophysiology. (1995) 32, no. 4, 373–381, 2-s2.0-0029025408, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01220.x.
- 67
Boutros N.,
Nasrallah H.,
Leighty R.,
Torello M.,
Tueting P., and
Olson S., Auditory evoked potentials, clinical vs. research applications, Psychiatry Research. (1997) 69, no. 2-3, 183–195, 2-s2.0-0030975462, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(96)02919-8.
- 68
Pfefferbaum A.,
Wenegrat B. G., and
Ford J. M., Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related potentials. II. Dementia, depression and schizophrenia, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1984) 59, no. 2, 104–124, 2-s2.0-0021264568.
- 69
Linden D. E. J., The P300: where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us?, Neuroscientist. (2005) 11, no. 6, 563–576, 2-s2.0-27744593500, https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858405280524.
- 70
Courchesne E.,
Hillyard S. A., and
Galambos R., Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1975) 39, no. 2, 131–143, 2-s2.0-0016720284.
- 71
Alho K., [email protected], Winkler I.,
Escera C.,
Huotilainen M.,
Virtanen J.,
Jääskeläinen I. P.,
Pekkonen E., and
Ilmoniemi R. J., Processing of novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex: magnetoencephalographic recordings, Psychophysiology. (1998) 35, no. 2, 211–224, https://doi.org/10.1017/S004857729800211X.
- 72
Baudena P.,
Halgren E.,
Heit G., and
Clarke J. M., Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1995) 94, no. 4, 251–264, 2-s2.0-0028934995, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(95)98476-O.
- 73
Knight R. T.,
Scabini D.,
Woods D. L., and
Clayworth C. C., Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3, Brain Research. (1989) 502, no. 1, 109–116, 2-s2.0-0024450523.
- 74
Mecklinger A. and
Ullsperger P., The P300 to novel and target events: a spatio-temporal dipole model analysis, NeuroReport. (1995) 7, no. 1, 241–245, 2-s2.0-0029549514.
- 75
Polo M. D.,
Escera C.,
Yago E.,
Alho K.,
Gual A., and
Grau C., Electrophysiological evidence of abnormal activation of the cerebral network of involuntary attention in alcoholism, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2003) 114, no. 1, 134–146, 2-s2.0-0037213047, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00336-X.
- 76
Lepistö T.,
Soininen M.,
Čeponiene R.,
Almqvist F.,
Näätänen R., and
Aronen E. T., Auditory event-related potential indices of increased distractibility in children with major depression, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2004) 115, no. 3, 620–627, 2-s2.0-1242292273, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.10.020.
- 77
Gumenyuk V., Electrophysiological and behavioral indices of distractibility in school-age children, Ph.D. thesis, 2005, Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland.
- 78
Kaipio M. L.,
Alho K.,
Winkler I.,
Escera C.,
Surma-Aho O., and
Näätänen R., Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries, NeuroReport. (1999) 10, no. 10, 2125–2129, 2-s2.0-0033551566.
- 79
Kaipio M. L.,
Cheour M.,
Čeponiené R.,
Öhman J.,
Alku P., and
Näätänen R., Increased distractibility in closed head injury as revealed by event- related potentials, NeuroReport. (2000) 11, no. 7, 1463–1468, 2-s2.0-0034657459.
- 80
Juckel G.,
Clotz F.,
Frodl T.,
Kawohl W.,
Hampel H.,
Pogarell O., and
Hegerl U., Diagnostic usefulness of cognitive auditory event-related P300 subcomponents in patients with Alzheimers disease?, Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. (2008) 25, no. 3, 147–152, 2-s2.0-44649134612, https://doi.org/10.1097/WNP.0b013e3181727c95.
- 81
Yamaguchi S., [email protected], Tsuchiya H.,
Yamagata S.,
Toyoda G., and
Kobayashi S., Event-related brain potentials in response to novel sounds in dementia, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2000) 111, no. 2, 195–203, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00228-X.
- 82
Näätänen R.,
Gaillard A. W. K., and
Mäntysalo S., Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted, Acta Psychologica. (1978) 42, no. 4, 313–329, 2-s2.0-0017991008.
- 83
Näätänen R. and
Michie P. T., Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: a critical review and reinterpretation, Biological Psychology. (1979) 8, no. 2, 81–136, 2-s2.0-0018778919, https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(79)90053-X.
- 84
Hari R.,
Hamalainen M., and
Ilmoniemi R., Responses of the primary auditory cortex to pitch changes in a sequence of tone pips: neuromagnetic recordings in man, Neuroscience Letters. (1984) 50, no. 1–3, 127–132, 2-s2.0-0021278447.
- 85
Scherg M.,
Vajsar J., and
Picton T. W., A source analysis of the late human auditory evoked potentials, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. (1989) 1, no. 4, 336–355, 2-s2.0-0024812746.
- 86
Alain C.,
Woods D. L., and
Knight R. T., A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans, Brain Research. (1998) 812, no. 1-2, 23–37, 2-s2.0-0032561721, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00851-8.
- 87
Näätänen R., Mismatch negativity: clinical research and possible applications, International Journal of Psychophysiology. (2003) 48, no. 2, 179–188, 2-s2.0-0038751935, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00053-9.
- 88
Näätänen R.,
Paavilainen P.,
Rinne T., and
Alho K., The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review, Clinical Neurophysiology. (2007) 118, no. 12, 2544–2590, 2-s2.0-36549081532, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026.
- 89
Näätänen R. and
Escera C., Mismatch negativity: clinical and other applications, Audiology and Neuro-Otology. (2000) 5, no. 3-4, 105–110, 2-s2.0-0342369392.
- 90
Czigler I.,
Csibra G., and
Csontos A., Age and inter-stimulus interval effects on event-related potentials to frequent and infrequent auditory stimuli, Biological Psychology. (1992) 33, no. 2-3, 195–206, 2-s2.0-0026733202, https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(92)90031-O.
- 91
Woods D. L., Auditory selective attention in middle-aged elderly subjects: an event-related brain potential study, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1992) 84, no. 5, 456–468, 2-s2.0-0026784654, https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(92)90033-8.
- 92
Pekkonen E.,
Rinne T.,
Reinikainen K.,
Kujala T.,
Alho K., and
Näätänen R., Aging effects on auditory processing: an event-related potential study, Experimental Aging Research. (1996) 22, no. 2, 171–184, 2-s2.0-0029979086.
- 93
Pekkonen E.,
Jousmaki V.,
Partanen J., and
Karhu J., Mismatch negativity area and age-related auditory memory, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. (1993) 87, no. 5, 321–325, 2-s2.0-0027491807, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(93)90185-X.
- 94
Kazmerski V. A.,
Friedman D., and
Ritter W., Mismatch negativity during attend and ignore conditions in Alzheimer′s disease, Biological Psychiatry. (1997) 42, no. 5, 382–402, 2-s2.0-0030819391, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00344-7.
- 95
Pekkonen E., Mismatch negativity in aging and in Alzheimer′s and Parkinson′s disease, Audiology and Neuro-Otology. (2000) 5, no. 3-4, 216–224, 2-s2.0-0034074010.
- 96
Pekkonen E.,
Jousmaki V.,
Kononen M.,
Reinikainen K., and
Partanen J., Auditory sensory memory impairment in Alzheimer′s disease: an event-related potential study, NeuroReport. (1994) 5, no. 18, 2537–2540, 2-s2.0-0028566219.
- 97
Pekkonen E.,
Jääskeläinen I. P.,
Erkinjuntti T.,
Hietanen M.,
Huotilainen M.,
Ilmoniemi R. J., and
Näätänen R., Preserved stimulus deviance detection in Alzheimer′s disease, NeuroReport. (2001) 12, no. 8, 1649–1652, 2-s2.0-0035854101.
- 98
Brønnick K. S.,
Nordby H.,
Larsen J. P., and
Aarsland D., Disturbance of automatic auditory change detection in dementia associated with Parkinson′s disease: a mismatch negativity study, Neurobiology of Aging. (2010) 31, no. 1, 104–113, 2-s2.0-70449517526, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.02.021.
- 99
Savolainen S.,
Karhu J.,
Pääkkönen A.,
Paljärvi L.,
Partanen J.,
Alafuzoff I., and
Vapalahti M., Auditory event-related potentials differentiate patients with normal pressure hydrocephalus and patients with concomitant Alzheimer′s disease verified by brain biopsy, NeuroReport. (2001) 12, no. 1, 33–37, 2-s2.0-0035931542.