Volume 13, Issue 1 pp. 53-76
Full Access

Transactional and Contextual Invalidation between the Parents of Disturbed Families: A Comparative Study

SUSANA ALICIA HASSAN M.A.

SUSANA ALICIA HASSAN M.A.

Psychology Clinic, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, California.

Search for more papers by this author
First published: March 1974
Citations: 13

The author gratefully acknowledges the valuable suggestions from Dr. Margaret T. Singer, and Dr. Paul Watzlawick. Dr. Silvia Sussman's “blind” ratings using the author's scoring system and Dr. Claude Guillier's statistical consultation substantially contributed to this study as well.

Abstract

This study was based on a standard task — the discussion of the question, “How, out of all the millions of people in the world, did the two of you get together?”— administered to the parents in “disturbed” and “non-disturbed” families. The family disturbance in the 46 families was classified according to its presence or absence, its degree, and its kind. The diagnoses were: schizophrenias (S), delinquency (D), under-achievement and/or enuresis, or psychosomatic complaints (U), ulcerative colitis (UC), and non-disturbed (N). Seven scales were used to score the parents' verbal exchanges. Three scales that measure transactional and contextual validation and invalidation significantly differentiated the groups. Mutual validation was maximal in the N couples and minimal in the D and S couples. Invalidation between the D and S couples appeared to arise from their unsuccessful metacommunications.

INTRODUCTION

The present research studied interaction patterns in 46 families with different psychopathologies and in “non-disturbed” families as well. An analysis was made of parental conversations around one standard question, “How, out of all the millions of people in the world, did the two of you get together?”

Seven scales were used to score the parental conversations. Three of these scales were quantitative ones. Scales A and B (“Number of Lines Spoken” and “Percentage of Participation”) were traditional quantifications. Scale C (“Order-Disorder”) assessed the regulation of the speech flow. The remaining four scales assessed more theoretical aspects of “informational exchange.” Scale D (“Interactional Positions”) classed control patterns. Scales E, F, and G (“Continuity-Discontinuity,”“The Opening Gambit,” and “Relations between the Spouses' Stories”) operationalized “transactional” and “contextual” validation and invalidation between two speakers.

Transactional and Contextual Invalidation

Of all the above measurements, the transactional and contextual invalidation scales differentiated highly among a five-fold grouping of families along the dimension of severity of disturbance within the family. It was assumed that singular transactions and total contextual sets either validate or invalidate the relationship context between two people. Implicit congruency between the logical types in one transaction or the total context validates or confirms the other. Hence, any shift or incongruency at the transactional and/or contextual levels requires a cognitive re-grouping whereby one can “make sense” of potentially paradoxical situations. The pragmatics of validation and invalidation can be described by discerning “successful” metacommunications from “unsuccessful” ones.

A “successful” metamessage is validating, since it entails adequate grasp of the framework in which meaning is to be interpreted. This enables the interactors to implement their mutual feedback for self-correction. “Successful” metacommunication includes paralinguistic cues of acknowledgment (“Mhm,”“Aha,” tones of voice), “creative solutions” (i.e., humor and fantasy), and comments on the conflicting levels of a message. In contrast, an “unsuccessful” metamessage is invalidating for it involves “internal” mislabeling of the other's intentions, feelings, and expectations. This distortion of interpersonal events in turn blurs the meaning of the exchanges and perplexes the interactors. Such distortion occurs systematically in families that experience the genuine expression of hostility, anger, and love as potentially catastrophic.

The above considerations reflect the interactional model of psychopathology, which originally focused on “schizophrenogenic” communication. Later investigators explored other disturbances, such as delinquency, underachievement, and ulcerative colitis. There are already some comparative studies of family interaction in relation to different psychiatric syndromes in one or more family members. This seems to be a fruitful direction which the present study followed as well.

METHOD

This study analyzed verbatim transcripts of 46 pairs of parents discussing the question, “How, out of all the millions of people in the world, did the two of you get together?” There were families with at least one “disturbed” member as well as “non-disturbed” families. This research assessed quantitative and qualitative dimensions of the parents' discourse in order to find out whether specific interaction styles were related to different intrafamilial psychopathologies. The parents' conversations were scored using seven interactional scales.

The research procedures will be described in the following order: (a) the sample, (b) the task, and (c) the scoring method.

The Sample

The couples were originally studied at the Mental Research Institute (M.R.I.) in Palo Alto, California, during the early sixties. The selected families were white, native-born, middle- and upper-middle class residents of the Palo Alto area. In most cases both biological parents were living together with the offspring; in a few instances one of the spouses was a step-parent. The children were under 21 years of age. The spouses' mean age was 40 years (range 38–53 for husbands and 27–55 for wives).

To follow are: the re-sampling criteria, the classification of the families, the severity-of-disturbance divisions, and the limitations of this sample.

The Re-sampling Criteria

After having listened carefully to the 65 available tapes, the author defined objective criteria of exclusion, which were related both to the interviews themselves and to the focus of this study. Thus, all the interviews that were discarded had at least one of four characteristics:

  • (a) 

    There was not a minimum of one scorable transaction along all the scales. This occurred for either of the following reasons. The spouses disregarded the instructions and interacted with the interviewer instead of talking with each other, or the spouses' exchange was too brief and sketchy.

  • (b) 

    The tape was mostly inaudible, due to technical defects.

  • (c) 

    The files were not available; hence, diagnosis remained unknown. The scoring was done “blind,” but knowledge of the clinical diagnosis was necessary in order to group the couples for the statistical analysis.

  • (d) 

    The couples had defined their problems as “marital difficulties,” and this study was limited to “family disturbances.”

The Classification of the Families Selected

Staff members at the M.R.I. drew largely upon the family's presenting complaints. Those families labeled “disturbed” reported problems in one or more family members. In contrast, no member of the “control” or “non-disturbed” families exhibited symptoms requiring therapy. In 42 of the 46 families reported here, the index patient was an offspring; in 4 cases the index patient was a parent. In 6 families there was more than one symptomatic individual (see Table I). Whenever there was more than one “identified patient,” a family was classified on the basis of the most severely disturbed member. Thus, on the whole the most severe symptomatic disturbance appeared in offspring and was, therefore, assumed to be the hallmark of the family.

Table 1. Distribution of the Cases in the Sample (N = 46)
Non- Mildly Disturbed Severely Disturbed
Disturbed
Group 1 2 3 4 5
normal underachievement ulcerative colitis delinquency schizophrenia
minor maladjustment
enuresis
psychosomatic complaints
Number of disturbances
   none 6
   one 7 underachievement 5 11 3 mothers
1 minor maladjustment 1 father
1 psychosomatic complaints 5 offspring
more than one 1 underachievement + 1 + under- 1 + under-
   psychosomatic complaints    achievement    achievement
2 underachievement 1 + under-achieve-
  + enuresis    ment + enuresis
      Total 6 12 6 13 9

Theoretically, if one considers that the family is a system whose patterns stem from contributions of the “sickest” member as well as the “healthiest,” it hardly matters where the pathology is “located.” Most family studies to date, it must be noted, tend to select the family's diagnosis on the basis of the index offspring. Accordingly, this was the criterion for classifying the families at the M.R.I. It is impressive that the present results utilizing the author's approach and those studies selecting the diagnosis of one offspring seem to have yielded congruent results.

The Severity-of-Disturbance Divisions in the Sample

Three different severity-of-disturbance divisions of the total sample were compared here. First, the comparison was between “non-disturbed” and “disturbed.” Second a three-group classification was made: (a) “non-disturbed”; (b) “mildly disturbed” (underachievement, minor maladjustment, enuresis, unspecified psychosomatic complaints, and ulcerative colitis); and (c) “severely disturbed” (delinquency and schizophrenias). Third, in a five-group classification, group 2 was subdivided into “mild family disturbances” and “families containing an ulcerative colitis patient.” Group 3 was composed of families containing a “delinquent” and those with a “schizophrenic” member. The five-group classification was inspired by specific findings on ulcerative colitis (49, 58) and on delinquency (7, 32, 51, 52). In succeeding pages the groups of the five-fold classification will be called groups 1 through 5.

The Limitations of the Sample

The present sample has certain limitations. Qualitatively, there is no matching for age, sex, or chronicity of the disturbances. Moreover, some families have a mixed pathology. Quantitatively, the design is unbalanced. In the dichotomous classification there are 6 control couples and 40 “disturbed” ones. The distribution in the three-fold grouping of “non-disturbed,”“mildly disturbed,” and “severely disturbed” is 6, 18, and 22 couples, respectively. The cases are more evenly distributed in the five-group classification. As Table I indicates, there are 6 “non-disturbed,” 12 “mildly disturbed,” 6 “ulcerative colitis,” 13 “delinquent,” and 9 “schizophrenic” index patients and their families.Table I shows the subgrouping within each of the major classes, which is based on the presence of none, one, or more than one kind of disturbance within the index patient.

In spite of the above drawbacks there are at least three strengths that lend validity to this sample. First, sociocultural factors were controlled, thus avoiding the socio-linguistic controversy (39). Second, the diagnoses were carefully made by skilled therapists at M.R.I. Third, the scoring was “blind” and independently done by two well-trained raters.

The Task

One of the five structured tasks in Watzlawick's project (56) was studied here. The question was, “How, out of all the millions of people in the world, did the two of you get together?” The interviewer asked this question without looking at either spouse, but pointing at both of them with a simultaneous gesture of both hands. He or she remained in the room listening to the couple's dialogue. The purpose of the question was to evoke themes concerning the couple's getting together and marrying. Although M.R.I. researchers found that this question elicited intriguing interactions, their analysis remained intuitive. No systematic scoring or statistical treatment was undertaken prior to the present one. The raters used in the present study were not involved in the interviewing or in the clinical assessment.

The “Meet” question provides a significant, conflict-solving task. It allows for a comfortable distance in time (“Meeting” took place somewhere in the past), while requiring the spouses to establish a shared focus of discussion around a common experience. Hence, the couples display ways in which they attempt to reach agreement, as well as their handling of disagreements. It also reflects the manner in which they utilize and distribute the interaction time. Although the “Meet” task takes place in a short period of time, there is already evidence that four to five minutes of family or dyadic conversation can reveal the usual patterns of an interactive system (18, 36, 56).

The Scoring Procedures

Three aspects of the couples' production were rated: 1) their productivity, 2) the unitary transactions, and 3) the overall impact of their interaction. There is a total of seven scales, which were lettered A through G. Figure 1 presents an outline of the scoring system; one interview is reproduced in Table II as illustration.

Outline of the Scoring System: The Seven Scales Utilized.

Table 2. Analysis of Transcript of One Interview
image image

1. Productivity Scales

A. Number of Lines Spoken: The total number of lines in the type-script were counted for each interview.

B. Percentage of Participation: The total lines spoken by the husband were divided by the total for the couple to obtain percentage scores. The wife's score was the residual of 100 per cent.

2. Unitary Transaction Scales

A transaction was defined here as “the relation of one, usually contiguous message, to another” (46). The interactional methodology hypothesizes a structural relationship between two, usually consecutive, messages by two persons. Hence, the author designated each transaction by bracketing the proper lines in the transcripts so that subsequent ratings would be made on the same segments of interaction. Thus, in the interaction between a husband (H) and a wife (W), the units are counted as follows,

and so on. Each unitary transaction was scored using scales C, D, E, and F.

C. Order-Disorder: This nominal scale (47) classed the information flow. Each transaction has one of five possible qualities: (1) “contiguity,” (2) “partial overlapping,” and (3) “non-disruptive total overlapping,” which are subcategories of order, whereas (4) “interruption” and (5) “disruptive total overlapping” are subcategories of disorder.

D. Interactional Positions: The positions of the interactors can be either (a) “symmetrical,” that is, tending toward the equal status of the participants, or (b) “complementarity,” i.e., differentiating between a “one-up” and a “one-down” party (2, 11, 45, 57).

E. Continuity-Discontinuity: This scale assumes that “every communicational act… may be viewed in some form as an agreement or a disagreement” (23) provided it ensures the “continuity” of the interactional sequence. The continuity categories are: (a) “agreement” (“simple agreement,”“amplification,” or “continuation”), which indicates convergent views; (b) “partial agreement” or “partial disagreement,” which expresses agreement with part of a message and disagreement with the rest of it; and (c) “disagreement” (“negation” or “correction”), which denotes discordant views. These three categories reflect different ways in which the interactors mutually acknowledge each other's messages. On the other hand, the discontinuity category, i.e., (d) “transactional disqualification,” applies when a person fails to label, or mislabels, a “discontinuity” in the interactive sequence. A “discontinuity” is thus introduced by either disregarding the other's message or by uttering a paradoxical one. To quote Sluzki, et al.: “If metacommunicative indicators are absent and the content is incongruent with the context, or if these indicators are present but the content is incongruent with them, there is a disqualification of message a by message b (46). Such transactions suggest avoidance of the decision to agree or disagree with the other. The subtypes of “transactional disqualification” considered are: “evasion,”“sleight-of-hand,”“status disqualification,” and “redundant question.”

Briefly: the continuity-discontinuity scale contains ten subcategories, all of which are mutually exclusive.

F. The Opening Gambit: The first transaction is thought to reflect the extent to which the speaker accepts the task's set. The initial utterance, by either spouse, in response to the interviewer's standard question is scored on a four-point scale: (a) “straight answer” involves directly answering, without any references to the question itself; (b) “labeling and/or repeating the question” involves positive or negative remarks about the question, cues of acknowledgment, and verification of adequate reception of the question (e.g., by echoing it); (c) “asking or giving one's spouse permission to go first” entails claiming or granting the right to speak; and (d) “question response” includes those responses that disconfirm the interviewer's request for an answer (43).

3. The Total Story Scale

G. Relations between the Spouses' Stories: Both spouses' participation was evaluated as contributions to a common product and scored accordingly. The raters drew upon their familiarity with the communicational features used for scoring T.A.T. and Rorschach protocols described by Singer and Wynne (42, 43). Two central features are “closure” (i.e., the joint construction of a meaningful story) and “coordination” (i.e., the ability to share the other's focus of attention and to participate together) between the husband's and the wife's statements. Each rater thus assessed a couple's total interaction, as in a conjoint T.A.T., or Rorschach, in terms of how closely it resembled one of six possible patterns. A numerical score from 1 to 6 was assigned on the basis of the kind and degree of deterioration in “closure” and “coordination”: 1) “concordance” reflects the predominance of different subtypes of “agreement”; 2) “discordance → concordance” refers to those cases in which the spouses begin disagreeing but are able to reach consensus; 3) “discordance” denotes the predominance of the different forms of “disagreement”; 4) “concordance → disconnection” includes those cases in which the spouses begin agreeing but end up with disconnected versions; 5) “disconnection” designates the prevalence of parallel messages; and 6) “contextual blurring” describes those conversations that are marked by lack of “closure” and lack of “coordination” as well. In contextual blurring there is an obliteration of the sense of meaningfulness, tangentializations reccur, and dubious logic is used; the whole interactional context is thus invalidated (43).

Reliability and Validity of the Scales

The scoring categories considered in each scale were mutually exclusive and, by and large, exhaustive. Each rater applied one scale at a time to the whole sample. Upon comparison of the results thus obtained by the two independent raters, the overall “blind” reliability was 80 per cent. The discordant items were then discussed between the two raters, thus reaching a final agreement of 95 per cent.

The percentage of non-scorable transactions was computed for all scales: 1 per cent on “Order-Disorder,” 9 per cent on “Continuity-Discontinuity,” and 15 per cent on “Interactional Positions.”“The Opening Gambit” and “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” had no non-scorables. “Number of Lines Spoken” and “Percentage of Participating” consisted of numerical computations.

RESULTS

The data analysis yielded overall significant results: p < .0001 for the three-group classification and p» .002 for the five-group one. The description of the computation of scores and the statistical treatments is beyond the scope of this paper. (A detailed report on the data analysis may be obtained directly from the author.)

In the following paragraphs, the statistical results will be translated into substantive findings. The five groups studied were compared by inspecting their typical scores in clusters of the significant variables. There were two clusters, which correspond to quantitative, versus qualitative, differentiations within the sample.

Quantitative Differentiations among the Five Groups Studied

Three dimensions —“Transactional Disqualification,”“The Opening Gambit,” and “Relations between the Spouses' Stories”— increased with the severity of the “family disturbance.” Only “Transactional Disqualification (couple)” isolated the controls; their lower disqualification scores separated them tenuously from the “mildly disturbed” groups (p» .15 with respect to groups 2 and 3) but sharply from the “severely disturbed” ones (p values of < .004 and » .05 for groups 4 and 5, respectively).

Impairment in “The Opening Gambit” (i.e., “Question Responses”) and in the “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” (i.e., lack of “closure” and “coordination”) proved useful in distinguishing groups 4 and 5 from all the others. These two scales assessed disqualifications of the interactional context as such, which only implicitly disqualified the unitary transactions. Thus, some interviews that failed to show any “Transactional Disqualifications,” nonetheless, had a couple of inappropriate statements that sufficed to dismiss the whole conversation. These “contextual” disqualifications entailed a massive though subtle deterioration in the transmission of meanings.

Qualitative Differentiations among the Five Groups Studied

The qualitative clustering stemmed from the observation that the scores on “Agreement,”“Disagreement (wife),” and “Complementarity One-Up (wife)” taken together produced a different pattern for each of the five groups studied.

“Agreement” is often considered to be related to the ability to talk things through and reach consensus, but its overuse may suggest defenses against discord. Thus high “Agreement” in the “ulcerative colitis” group —“agreement by withdrawal” in Sojit's words (50) — may help account for their expression of tensions through bodily symptoms. Conversely, low “Agreement” usually reflects the inability to share a view, a lack of mutuality, as in delinquentogenic and schizophrenogenic families (groups 4 and 5). Low “Agreement,” high “Transactional Disqualification,” and high scores in “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” were common features of these two groups.

In view of their very marginal significance (p» .15), “Disagreement (wife)” and “Complementarity One-Up (wife)” should be taken as indicative of trends within the sample. The controls had both high “Disagreement” and “Agreement” and low “Transactional Disqualification.” On the other hand, group 4 showed a pattern of high “Disagreement” combined with high “Transactional Disqualification” and low “Agreement.” To this was added highest “Complementarity One-Up (wife),” which suggests a touch of one-upmanship between the spouses in group 4. In contrast with group 4, group 5 showed low “Disagreement.” Nevertheless, groups 4 and 5 resembled each other in that they both had low “Agreement” and high “Transactional Disqualification” scores. Therefore, different processes can be assumed to underlie the combination of scores in groups 4 and 5. On one hand, the ensemble of scores in group 4 suggests isolation and interpersonal distance (19, 32), a “self-centered” attitude (51), or “disconnection” (13). On the other hand, the typical scores of group 5 indicate the inability of members of a schizophrenogenic family to either disagree or to show accord. They thus become deeply “embroiled” with one another (14). Another important finding is that, although both group 5 and 3 had low “Disagreement” scores, the “costs” of avoiding arguments seem different in those two groups. A more workable solution in group 3 is reflected by low “Transactional Disqualification” as well as high “Agreement”. The typical scores for each of the five groups studied are illustrated in Figure 2.

Typical Scores of the Five Groups in the Significant Dimensions.

DISCUSSION

The Empirical Conclusions

This research discriminated among the five groups studied, and the results obtained raised issues related to (a) the interpretation of intragroup variability; (b) the differentiation between the “delinquentogenic” and the “schizophrenogenic” parents; and (c) the supersummatory effects of parents.

The Interpretation of Intragroup Variability

There was high variability within groups 2 (“underachievement,”“minor maladjustment,”“psychosomatic complaints,”“enuresis”) and 5 (“schizophrenias”). The heterogeneity in group 2 supported Stierlin's finding (52) that “underachievement” appears in different kinds of families. In order to identify the underachievers' families that are at high risk for severe disturbances one must look at the specific interaction style between an underachiever's parents.

The variability within group 5 has a twofold explanation. First, it suggests the separation of “childhood schizophrenia” (CS) from “young adult schizophrenia” (YAS). The YAS parents exhibited high “Transactional Disqualification” scores. Contrarily, the CS parents obtained for the most part “Continuity” scores, but their conversations showed at some point a couple of irrelevant speeches or an inappropriate tone that destroyed the integrity of the plot. Hence, these two subgroups shared the end product scored “Contextual Blurring” via different processes. This confirms the differentiation between CS and YAS parents, reported by Singer and Wynne (42).

A second factor of variability in group 5 was the combination of psychological and neurological pathologies in the index patient in one family. This was suggested by the finding of only one significant error in the “Discriminant Function Analysis” of a group 5 couple, who thus appeared similar to the “controls.” This couple had a teenage son who had been successively diagnosed as “mentally retarded” and “schizophrenic.” This enhances Goldfarb's (8) finding that parents who have an offspring with “soft signs” of neurological impairment tend to appear quite intact emotionally. Indeed, such families should be excluded from studies like the present one.

The Differentiation between “Delinquentogenic” and “Schizophrenogenic” Parents

The differentiation between “delinquentogenic” (D) and “schizophrenogenic” (S) parents was suggested mainly by the “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” scale (G). The D parents frequently exhibited “Disconnection,” whereas the S parents showed mostly “Contextual Blurring” in their stories. Additionally, it seemed that the subdivision of “Transactional Disqualification” into “parallel” transactions and “antinomic” ones may help discriminate between the D and S parents at the transactional level as well. The D parents often responded to each other in a “parallel” way, whereas the S parents engaged in paradoxical exchanges. These characteristics appear to be related to Reiss' (35) finding that D parents tend to be “interpersonal-distance-sensitive,” whereas S parents usually bemuddle each other by being excessively “consensus-sensitive.”

The Supersummatory Effects of the Marital Couple

The comparison of “Husband” and “Wife” scores versus the “Couple” scores lends support to the hypothesis that the joint impact of the marital couple is different (“supersummatory”) from the separate effect of each spouse. The analysis of the separate scores showed “Wife” scores to be more uniform than the “Husband” scores within each group and more variable across groups. In contrast, “Husband” scores exhibited a higher intragroup dispersion. This helped explain the overlap of “Husband” scores across groups in some variables that had reached significance at the “Couple” level. Moreover, this finding suggested that the wife's role in marking the couple's style might be more definitive than the husband's. In terms of marital roles one could then speculate that the mother might be the intermediary between the tensions in the marital subsystem and the family.

Another relevant finding was that the separate “Husband” and “Wife” scores suggested a compensatory mechanism in the “mildly disturbed” groups (2 and 3) but not in the “severely disturbed” ones (groups 4 and 5). For instance, “Transactional Disqualification” for husbands and wives differed in group 2. Conversely, those two scores resembled each other in groups 4 and 5. This supports the hypothesis that the least disturbed of two spouses can attenuate the family disturbance (42, 29).

Theoretical Recapitulation of the Findings

Scales A (“Number of Lines Spoken”), B (“Percentage of Participation”), and C (“Order-Disorder”) failed to show any differences among the five groups studied. Scale D only hinted at a differentiation within the sample (p» .15). On the other hand, scales E (Continuity-Discontinuity”), F (“The Opening Gambit”), and G (“Relations between the Spouses' Stories”) distinguished well among the five groupings of families.

In view of the above results, scales A, B, C, and D will not be further examined. The focus will be on the three significant scales: E, F, and G. Hence, validation and invalidation will be considered within the framework of two major constructs: (a) “logical typing” of messages and (b) “transactional thought disorders”.

Validation-Invalidation and the “Logical Typing” of Messages

In the early fifties, Bateson introduced Whitehead and Russell's theory of logical types (4) into the study of communication disturbances. Bateson conceived of each message in terms of two distinct aspects, namely “primary communication” and “meta-communication.”“Metacommunication” is of a higher logical type than “primary communication” or the “content” aspect, for “metacommunication” comments (implicitly and/or explicitly) on the communicative process, thereby defining the relationship context. Bateson and his co-workers were intrigued with those incongruencies between logical types that generate a paradoxical relationship pattern, the “double-bind” being its paradigm. In line with Bateson's scheme, it was assumed that “metacommunication” at either the transactional or contextual levels can validate or invalidate a given exchange. Accordingly, the focus here was on two metacommunicative units — i.e., the “singular transaction” and the “total context.”

In the present study, the singular transaction was scored by the “Continuity-Discontinuity” scale (E), whose categories assessed the step-by-step decision-making as the conversation progressed. Each exchange between two partners calls for a decision about the other's message. This decision appears in statements that express decidability, that is, any subcategory of “Agreement,” or “Partial Agreement,” or “Disagreement.” Conversely, the cases of unlabeled or mislabeled undecidability were scored “Transactional Disqualification.”“Transactional Disqualification” should not be regarded as either implicit agreement or disagreement, since it lacks clear cues of acknowledgment of the previous message. There is no apparent decision about the message. Undecidability, however, does not always nor necessarily result in disqualifications. Therefore, the introduction of an additional category is now suggested for future scoring of those ways of handling undecidability and contextual switches that do preserve the meaningfulness of the relationship context (i.e., mutual validation). The proposed category would permit more clear-cut differentiation between “successful” and “unsuccessful” metacommunication. This study showed the close link between “unsuccessful” metacommunication and “transactional disqualification.”

The total contextual set entails rules between the interactors that can be inferred from the prevalence as well as the absence of certain classes of singular transactions. The emphasis here was on styles of sharing information along three dimensions. The first two, namely, the transmission of meaning (“Closure” versus “Disconnection” or “Contextual Blurring”) and the mutuality of the exchanges (“Coordination” versus “Pseudomutuality”) were assessed by the “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” scale (G). Thus, “Concordance,”“Discordance → Concordance,” and “Discordance” reflect adequate ways of coordinating viewpoints toward a sense of “closure.” Conversely, “Concordance → Disconnection,”“Disconnection,” and “Contextual Blurring” reinforce confusion and “pseudomutuality” (60). Thirdly, the expectations about the interpersonal situation were scored by “The Opening Gambit” scale (F) in terms of the validation (“Straight Answer” or “Labeling and/or Repetition of the Question” or “Asking or Giving Permission to Go First”) or the invalidation (“Question Response”) of the task as a whole.

Validation-Invalidation and “Transactional Thought Disorders”

In the early sixties, Wynne and Singer (61, 62) proposed an “epigenetic and transactional” approach to “thought disorders,” which points to deficiencies in concept formation within affectively toned relationship contexts — i.e., expressiveness among family members. Through the daily interactions with his parents, the child normally develops cognitive skills that allow him or her to interpret interpersonal situations. According to the Wynne-Singer team, then, validation (“mutuality”) would reflect the ability to abstract the relevant properties from either a particular transaction or the total context. This cognitive process presupposes shared focal attention between the inter-actors so that they can in turn monitor and label each other's intentions, feelings, and expectations “internally.” Only thus can any two persons engage in meaningful communication.

Wynne and Singer found that a disordered attentional set (“amorphous” or “fragmented” or “coherent delusional”) (63) toward the listener blurs the meaning of the speaker's messages, thus invalidating the transactions and their context. The extreme forms of “thought disorders” are, in this sense, the schizophrenias. They are characterized by the alteration of symbolic functioning, essentially a breakdown in category formation in the area of social interaction. Yet, this impairment does not necessarily entail the degradation of intellectual potential — e.g., for the sciences and the arts, and instrumental performance in general (1, 15, 24, 25, 26).

To sum up: The emphasis of the Bateson group on “logical typing” of messages, as well as Wynne's and Singer's focus on attentional sets, constitute two relevant ways of conceptualizing the processes by which interactors validate their relationship, or fail to do so.

Empirically, the present study of “transactional” and “contextual” invalidation supports earlier research that has linked parents' communication style to their offspring's psychopathology (4, 7, 9, 10, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 27, 29, 32, 35, 37, 42, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 58, 61, 62). The author views the systematic focus on metacommunicative dimensions as a most promising line for further inquiry into the structure of family interaction.

Footnotes

  • Reprint requests should be addressed to Susana Alicia Hassan, 1543 Spruce St., No. 2, Berkeley, California 94709.
  • 1 In 4 of the “schizophrenic” families, the index patient was a parent — three mothers and one father. Such exceptions were made, since there were very few cases of “schizophrenia” in the original sample, and those 4 interviews qualified for inclusion here.
  • 2 A detailed Scoring Manual with its theoretical and empirical foundations is available upon request. Here only a brief example is given to illustrate each scale.
  • 3 In 4 out of the 9 families in group 5, a parent (3 mothers and 1 father) was the index patient. Since no differences were found between these 4 cases and the other 5, one can conclude that the 4 exceptions to the “offspring-as-patient” criterion (used in 42 out of the 46 families studied) did not alter the results obtained for group 5 (i.e., the “schizophrenias”).
  • 4 It is crucial to keep in mind that the singular transaction was the scoring unit in scale E in order to avoid confusions with the notion that “Agreement” can be related to “Disqualifications” that appear at a later point in a sequence (50). The latter are “contextual” invalidations, which were assessed in this study by the “Relations between the Spouses' Stories” scale (G).
    • The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.