Volume 41, Issue 4 pp. 848-891
Extended Article
Free Access

Transitivity, Space, and Hand: The Spatial Grounding of Syntax

Timothy W. Boiteau

Corresponding Author

Timothy W. Boiteau

Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina

Correspondence should be sent to Timothy W. Boiteau, Institute for Mind and Brain, University of South Carolina, 1800 Gervais St., Columbia, SC 29201. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this author
Amit Almor

Amit Almor

Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina

Linguistics Program, University of South Carolina

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 10 March 2016
Citations: 7

Abstract

Previous research has linked the concept of number and other ordinal series to space via a spatially oriented mental number line. In addition, it has been shown that in visual scene recognition and production, speakers of a language with a left-to-right orthography respond faster to and tend to draw images in which the agent of an action is located to the left of the patient. In this study, we aim to bridge these two lines of research by employing a novel method that measures the spatial bias produced by transitive sentences that use a wide variety of abstract and concrete verbs. Across four experiments, participants read sentences and then responded to probe words appearing on either the left or right sides of the screen. Probe words consisted of agents, patients, other words in the sentence, or newly encountered words. We found consistent lateral biases to responding to agents and patients, which appears to be independent of order of mention in the sentence but which does reflect a correspondence between position in the sentence and role in the causal sequence of the action. Our results also show that this spatial bias is driven by the use of the hands in two different ways: The left hand shows a greater sensitivity to the spatial effect than the right hand, and vocal responses produce no spatial effect.

1 Introduction

A central issue in cognitive psychology is how the mind processes language. Work by cognitive grammarians suggests the possibility that syntax, a crucial part of language, may involve an inherent spatial representation or prototypical temporal sequencing (Croft, 1991; Langacker, 2001; Talmy, 2000). While this research may seem non-central to psycholinguistic interests, connections between space and language have been made by psychologists, mainly in the context of metaphor (Boroditsky, 2001; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980), semantics (Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003), or explicitly spatial language (Levinson, 1996). One area in cognitive psychology where the connection between space and syntax has been addressed more directly is in studies of visual scene production (e.g., drawing) and recognition tasks (Chatterjee, 2001; Chatterjee, Southwood, & Basilico, 1999; Maas & Russo, 2003), which have found evidence for a spatial bias in verb representations such that participants recognize scenes with action motion trajectories moving left-to-right faster than those moving right-to-left. Interestingly, this lateral asymmetry is similar to findings from research on the relation between space and number (e.g., Bueti & Walsh, 2009; Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993; Hubbard, Piazza, Pinel, & Dehaene, 2005; Ishihara, Keller, Rossetti, & Prinz, 2008), the general conclusion from this research being that smaller quantities (e.g., low numbers, small grip aperture) prime the left side of space, and larger quantities (e.g., high numbers, large grip aperture) prime the right side. In this study, we build on these various lines of research to further explore the relationship between syntax and space.

The relation between language and space is closely related to questions of embodiment, namely the role of sensorimotor representations in language processing. Indeed, related lines of research have found evidence for the embodiment of language in motor processes (Glenberg & Kaschak, 2002; Hauk, Johnsrude, & Pulvermuller, 2004; Zwaan & Taylor, 2006) and visuospatial representations (e.g., Engelen, Bouwmeester, de Bruin, & Zwaan, 2011; Stanfield & Zwaan, 2001). For example, Glenberg, Sato, and Cattaneo (2008) had participants transfer several hundred beans one by one from one jar to another, moving either in a direction toward or away from the body and later had these participants make plausibility judgments of sentences with implied directional transfer. Participants were slower to respond to sentences with implied motion in the direction they had been moving the beans, indicating that overworked motor effectors were interfering with semantic processing. Other studies have shown that hand- and foot-based verbs modulate motor evoked potentials measured in the hands and feet, respectively (Buccino et al., 2005), that processing hand and foot action sentences differentially facilitates manual and pedal responses (Scorolli & Borghi, 2007), and that there is a benefit for responding to concrete words with the hands and abstract relations with the mouth (Granito, Scoroli, & Borghi, 2015). In addition to these motor embodiment effects, a number of studies have shown that language is also embodied in visuospatial processing (Stanfield & Zwaan, 2001; Zwaan, Stanfield, & Yaxley, 2002; Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003). Most relevant to this study is the study by Zwaan and Yaxley (2003), in which participants made semantic relatedness judgments to word pairs that were or were not related and which (among related pairs) were presented in either canonical or non-canonical vertical positions relative to one another. For example, one item (coffee and foam) presented coffee either below or above foam in canonical or non-canonical conditions, respectively. When word pairs were presented to the left visual field (and hence the right hemisphere), there was an effect of position, but when presentation was to the right visual field (left hemisphere), position had no effect.

Reminiscent of the work in embodied cognition, cognitive grammarians have argued that syntax reflects iconic properties of the world. Langacker (2001) proposed that word order, and thus the conceptual evolution of a linguistic utterance over time, is a central component in that utterance's meaning. Some important factors to consider about word order are how something is expressed (the surface structure used), the conceptual order, and the real-world ordering of events. Most languages tend to order words in such a way that these different factors line up. For example, of the six possible orderings of the main syntactic elements of a transitive sentence (i.e., subject (S), verb (V) and object (O)), SVO and SOV account for the basic word orders of roughly 90% of the world's languages (Dryer, 2006). These orderings may not be accidental but rather may iconically reflect the causal flow of events (Croft, 1991). Along similar lines, these orderings may be the product of constraints of language production (MacDonald, 2013), such that subjects may be accessed from memory and hence produced first because they are more salient figures in an action. Generally, these various accounts predict that the notion of time is an important aspect in linguistic meaning and is therefore tied into the very grammatical structure of a sentence. This provides a serendipitous marriage between the notion of language with those of magnitude and space (discussed further below). Relatedly, Talmy (2000) introduced the concept of force dynamics, in which all interactions are processed in a manner akin to how physical forces act upon one another. In any sentence there are agonists and antagonists, taking the roles of agents, patients, subjects, objects, etc. Verbs express motion, be it actual, metaphorical, or unrealized. This motion is a result of the struggle to action between the antagonist and agonist. This can apply to not only concrete but also abstract concepts, such as the verb want, which, according to Talmy's analysis, implies psychological pressure or force toward some goal. Thus, force dynamics evoke Lakoff and Johnson's (1980) proposal of the grounding of abstract concepts in the concrete as well as the more general notion of grounded cognition, where language comprehension is embodied in sensorimotor processes (e.g., Glenberg & Kaschak, 2002; Zwaan & Taylor, 2006). However, Talmy's proposal (and what we will argue for in this study) is that syntax is also in some sense grounded.

One of the first studies to show that there may indeed be an empirically observable relation between syntax and space came from Chatterjee, Maher, Gonzalez Rothi, and Heilman (1995). These authors described the case of a left-handed patient (WH) suffering from a right hemisphere stroke that left him agrammatic. In one of the experiments from that study WH listened to transitive sentences in the active or passive voice and selected between two pictures that either accurately or inaccurately depicted the action. The stimuli described circle and square cartoon characters, effectively eliminating animacy plausibility cues. While WH performed at chance for both active and passive sentences in this experiment, if the lateral direction of movement of the action was controlled for, it was found he performed at ceiling. Specifically, he performed accurately on active sentences when they described left-to-right action, and on passive sentences when they described right-to-left action. Thus, WH was using a combination of spatial and temporal information to reconcile the pictures with the sentences. Building upon the results from this patient study, Chatterjee et al. (1999) showed that when drawing images, healthy English-speaking participants tended to place the agent of the action on the left side of the picture. In addition, they were faster in matching sentences to images when the image depicted the agent of a transitive action on the left and the patient on the right. Maas and Russo (2003) tested whether the effects Chatterjee and colleagues had reported so far were an effect of the orthographic direction of the language spoken by participants (i.e., the left-to-right direction of English), comparing the performance of Italian and Arabic speakers on a sentence-picture matching task similar to the one used in Chatterjee et al.'s experiments. They found that Italian speakers responded more quickly to images with the agent on the left, and Arabic speakers to images depicting agents on the right, suggesting that indeed orthographic direction drives this effect. However, in addition to this finding, both language groups showed a benefit to responding to images with action moving in a left-to-right direction (e.g., push and pull with agents on the left and right sides, respectively), which may suggest that in addition to orthography, the neural lateralization of language is also contributing to the effect. Specifically, Chatterjee (2001) proposed that individuals with left-lateralized language may show facilitation in responding to left-to-right flowing action as a result of left hemispheric regions processing such types of motion (Kinsbourne, 1987). Indeed, left hemispheric regions are involved in launching contraversive saccades (Herter & Guitton, 2004) and manual movements (McNeil & Pedelty, 1995) toward the right region of space. Left-to-right motion processing may recruit occipital-temporal regions and inferior and superior parietal cortices in the left hemisphere (Wu, Morganti, & Chatterjee, 2008). While the studies by Chatterjee et al. and Maas and Russo suggest native language and language lateralization together lead to interactions between space and verbal processing, Altmann, Saleem, Kendall, Heilman, and Gonzales Rothi (2006), using a larger n (18) of verbs than Maas and Russo (4: give, take, push, and pull), failed to find an agent spatial bias or a verb directionality effect when testing both Arabic and English speakers. It is notable also that Chatterjee's experiments employed a low n of 12 verbs, leaving open the possibility that this spatial effect is driven by an idiosyncratic group of concrete verbs. Another problem is that all these studies employed methods involving either scene recognition (i.e., sentence-picture matching) or production (i.e., drawing), making it possible that language comprehenders do not automatically form these spatial representations but rather only do so when the task involves relating language to visual scenes.

In addition to these various hand-language and language-space connections, there is a clear relationship between space and hand. For example, the Simon effect (the faster responding to a stimulus that shares some task-irrelevant dimension with a response) has typically been elicited using tasks involving stimuli presented at discrete locations that match the lateralization of manual responses (e.g., Iani, Baroni, Pellicano, & Nicoletti, 2011; Rubichi & Nicoletti, 2006; Simon & Rudell, 1967; Simon & Small, 1969). Mental representations of space are impacted by our ability to manipulate surrounding stimuli, and prosthetic extensions of the hand that bring distant regions of space within reach alter the neural receptive field associated with those spatial regions (Colby & Goldberg, 1999; Iriki, Tanaka, & Iwamura, 1996). Indeed, some studies have shown that processing stimuli in near-hand space leads to greater contributions of the magnocellular versus parvocellular visual pathways (Chan, Peterson, Barense, & Pratt, 2013; Gozli, West, & Pratt, 2012). This effect has also been shown to occur in language, where semantic judgments are degraded in near-hand space (Davoli, Du, Montana, Gaverick, & Abrams, 2010). These studies thus suggest that the relation between hand and space is an important factor to consider when exploring the connection between linguistic and spatial processing.

Many studies have found an intimate connection between magnitude and space (i.e., the spatial-numerical association of response codes; SNARC; Dehaene et al., 1993; Hubbard et al., 2005). The association between number and space can be observed in numerous ways. For example, processing mathematical operation signs produce spatial biases such that addition primes the right and subtraction the left (Pinhas, Shaki, & Fischer, 2014). Bodily movement toward the left or right primes small or large numbers, respectively (Anelli, Lugli, Borghi, & Nicoletti, 2014; Hartmann, Grabherr, & Mast, 2012). Some studies have implicated the importance of the hand in the SNARC effect. For example, the typical SNARC effect disappears when the hands are crossed (Wood, Nuerck, & Willmes, 2006), and differences between finger counting habits both within (Fischer, 2008) and across (Domahs, Moeller, Huber, Wilmes, & Nuerk, 2010) cultures modulate the SNARC effect. It should be noted, however, that this effect has also been elicited via responding with the feet (Schwarz & Müller, 2006), suggesting that a combination of finger counting habits and response lateralization contributes to the SNARC effect. The SNARC is also modulated to some degree by the orthographic direction of the native language (Dehaene et al., 1993). Furthermore, over-learned ordinal sequences (e.g., letters of the alphabet and months) also produce a SNARC-like effect (Gevers, Reynvoet, & Fias, 2003). Together, these findings suggest that experience with finger counting and exposure to a left-to-right orthography over the course of development may lead to the left-small/right-large SNARC effect, but others have shown that similar associations exist in infants (de Hevia, Girelli, Addabbo, & Cassia, 2014) and domestic chicks (Rugani, Vallortigara, Priftis, & Regolin, 2015).

Other effects related to the SNARC that go beyond written sequences may also prove to be important in language processing. First, Ishihara et al. (2008) showed that judgments of stimulus onset (short vs. long) are made faster to the left than to the right, respectively (the spatial-temporal association of response codes; STEARC). This seems especially relevant to language, given the fact that the speech signal unfolds over (and thus is dependent on) time. In this sense, whether a language is written from left-to-right, right-to-left, or top-to-bottom, the STEARC effect would predict that words at the beginning of a sentence should evoke a more leftward bias than words appearing farther down the speech stream, based on their association with relative moments in time. Xuan, Zhang, He, and Chen (2007) also observed that more of something (e.g., 7 dots vs. 3 dots; bright lights vs. dim lights; big squares vs. small squares) is judged to last longer than less of something, an issue that may be important with regards to the relative saliency of entities in a discourse model. Finally, Nuerk, Iverson, and Wilmes (2004) found that congruent linguistic markedness leads to facilitation of responses, whereas incongruency produces interference. For example, in a parity judgment task (typical to the SNARC literature) when even numbers are mapped onto right-hand responses and odd numbers onto the left, there will be facilitation, as both even and right are the less marked words of the pairs even and odd and left and right (the linguistic markedness of response codes; MARC effect; see also Roettger & Domahs, 2015).

While these findings may seem a bit far removed from the study of syntax, as we already mentioned, language is necessarily processed through various mediums relying on space and time. These interactions could potentially create SNARC-like effects in language processing and may elucidate some of the spatial biases of language that we reviewed above. Specifically, the SNARC literature would predict that any word appearing at the start of a sentence should show a left-side bias and any word at the end should show a right-side bias (if we assume a sentence as being roughly comparable to a left-to-right mental number line). The reasons for this (in English) are that beginning words appear early in the speech stream and in a leftward position in the text. This might similarly apply to the subjects and objects (or agents and patients) of a sentence, as they usually occur in predictable sentence locations. Thus, research from a number of areas, including cognitive grammar, visual scene recognition, embodied cognition, and numerical processing, all suggest that syntax may have an underlying lateral spatial component. Furthermore, research on the relation between hand and space suggests that use of the hands may change the nature of spatial processing, for example via altering the weights of the visual processing pathways or the contribution of the left and right hemispheres. If such spatial representations do exist, they might be explained differently, depending on the theoretical framework of interpretation. A cognitive grammarian invoking Talmy's force dynamics might explain that successfully parsing a syntactic structure involves depicting entities acting upon one another. However, from the perspective of the magnitude processing literature, language might epiphenomenally interact with space due to developmental priming from reading (a visuospatial process in itself). The purpose of the subsequent four experiments is first to provide evidence for a connection between syntax, space, and hand (E1) and then to determine the nature of this effect (E2, E3, E4), possibly allowing us to tease apart the contributions of the various theoretical frameworks. Transitive sentences are an ideal tool for studying the underlying spatial nature of language, as they require two participants occurring in a predictable sequence. Similarly, they can be passivized to reverse this sequence. Thus, we tested various aspects of transitive sentences. In E1 we compare the lateral bias of subjects and objects. In E2 we ask whether the final word of a sentence shows a stronger rightward lateral bias than other words in the sentence. In E3 we manipulate sentence voice to see if passivization leads to a reversal in the lateral bias of agents and patients. Finally, in E4 we determine the importance of the hand in the results of the previous experiments.

2 Experiment 1

Experiment 1 was designed to extend findings from other research lines and establish them in a simple reading paradigm. Our main interest was to investigate the possible spatial bias of different parts of transitive sentences: the subject and the object. We wondered if participants would show a preference for identifying the subject of a sentence when it appeared on the left side of space and identifying the object of a sentence when it appeared on the right. For testing this hypothesis, we developed a design inspired by techniques used in the Simon and SNARC effects literature (Hubbard et al., 2005; Simon, 1969), using a button box with left- and right-lateralized buttons, and with participants responding to probes appearing in stimulus-response matching or mismatching regions of space after they read a sentence presented word by word at the center of the screen.

2.1 Methods

2.1.1 Participants

Forty-eight participants (45 female, 3 male, Mage = 20.5) took part in this study for extra credit toward a psychology class. All participants were right-handed (self-reported), native speakers of English with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (self-reported). Participants gave their informed consent to participate in this research under the guidelines of the University of South Carolina Institutional Review Board.

2.1.2 Procedure

Eighty transitive sentences were created, each consisting of two male or female human characters, with an equal number of all-female or all-male items (see Fig. 1 for a sample item and Table A1 for all stimuli used). The purpose of the gender manipulation was to prevent participants from forming any unconscious associations between gender and agency (e.g., male dominance associated with syntactic role dominance). Sentences were an average of 7.1 words long (range: 5–10). We equated word length of subject (Mlength = 5.41) and object (Mlength = 5.58) names within sentences such that on average possible probe words of a given sentence were of the same length (= 0.7, > .05). Sentences were presented one word at a time in the center of a computer monitor. Each trial began with a fixation cross lasting 1-s. This was followed by the sentence, 500 ms for each word with no delay between words. Following the final word, another fixation cross was presented for 1,500 ms. After this, a probe (either the subject or object of the previous sentence) appeared on either the left or right side of the screen. Participants were instructed to identify whether the probe was the subject or object of the previously read sentence and press a corresponding button on a PST Serial Response Box with a left button press when encountering the subject and a right button press for the object, and on the other half of trials button assignment was switched. Participants were told that the subject of the sentence was the person who appeared first in the sentence. Participants had 4,500 ms to respond to the probe before advancing to the next trial. Failures to respond were coded as incorrect. Items were divided into two blocks for the different button assignments, each beginning with eight practice trials presenting items in all possible conditions (Male vs. Female, Subject vs. Object, Left vs. Right side presentation). Participants could only advance into the experimental blocks after achieving at least 75% accuracy. Button assignment order was counterbalanced across subjects. Sentence presentation within block was randomized (see Fig. 1 for schematic representation of a sample trial and Appendix for a list of all items used across the experiments).

Details are in the caption following the image
Schematic of sample trials from all experiments.

2.2 Results

For this and subsequent experiments we are primarily interested in response times to correct probe recognition. Therefore, before conducting data analyses we removed all incorrect responses from the dataset (21.18%). In addition, we reasoned that participants were not able to fully process and generate responses for items faster than 300 ms, whereas responses slower than 3,000 ms reflected failures to attend to the task. Thus, responses that did not fall in between these limits were removed from the dataset as well (0.19%). In all, then, 21.37% of responses were removed, and log-transformed response times were analyzed.

Data analyses were conducted in the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler, & Bolker, 2011) in R (v.3.1.1; R Development Core Team, 2014), using mixed-effects modeling with stimulus Side (Left vs. Right), response Hand (LHand vs. RHand), and Word type (Subject vs. Object) as fixed effects, as well as all possible interactions between the three factors, and random intercepts and slopes both by-participants and by-items (Baayen, Davidson, & Bates, 2008; Barr, Levy, Scheepers, & Tily, 2013). The reason for including random slopes for participants and items is that there may be variability in the sensitivity of different participants and items to the fixed effects (Barr et al., 2013). We compared the full model with a model that included all two-way interactions and main effects and found that the full model was a better fit, χ2(1) = 7.82, = .005. Full model results are reported in Table 1 and shown in Fig. 2.

Table 1. Fixed effects for the max model in Experiment 1. Baseline level for Word is Object, for Side is Left, and for Hand is Left
Coefficient estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.655 0.032 206.382 <0.001
β1 Subject −0.1 0.018 −5.661 <0.001
β2 RightSide −0.00006 0.018 −0.003 ≤0.997
β3 RHand −0.054 0.018 −3.061 ≤0.002
β4 Subject × RightSide 0.046 0.025 1.832 ≤0.067.
β5 Subject × RHand 0.113 0.025 4.534 <0.001
β6 RightSide × RHand 0.014 0.025 0.546 ≤0.585
β7 Subject × RightSide × RHand −0.099 0.035 −2.798 ≤0.005

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.
Details are in the caption following the image
Graphical results of Experiment 1, showing the interaction between Side, Hand, and Word. Log-transformed RTs on the y-axis. Bars represent SE.

To get a better understanding of the three-way interaction, we ran a series of post hoc comparisons, adjusting p values using the Bonferroni method. Due to the large number of possible contrasts, we only report in the text those that were of theoretical interest (see Table 2 for all contrasts). First, there was a benefit for responding to Subjects on the Left using the LHand as opposed to Objects on the Left with the LHand, β = 0.1, SE = 0.018, = 5.66, < .001. In addition, responses to Subjects on the Left with the LHand were faster than to Objects on the Right with the LHand, β = −0.1, SE = 0.018, = −5.66, < .001. Responses to Subjects on the Left with the LHand were faster than responses to Subjects on the Left with the RHand, β = −0.06, SE = 0.018, = −3.33, = .02. Responses to Subjects on the Left with the LHand were also faster than responses to Objects on the Right with the RHand, β = −0.06, SE = 0.018, = −3.37, = .02. Responses to Subjects on the Right with the RHand were faster than to Objects on the Left with the LHand, β = 0.081, SE = 0.018, = 4.51, < .001. All other contrasts were not statistically significant.

Table 2. Pairwise comparisons for the three-way interaction between Side (Left-L, Right-R), Word (Subject-Sub, Object-Ob), and Hand (Right Hand-RH, Left Hand-LH) in E1, each cell reporting coefficient difference estimates, SE, t ratios, and p values
Condition Value LSubH LObRH LSubRH RObLH RSubLH RObRH RSubRH
LObLH Est. .1 .054 .041 <.001 .054 .041 .081
SE .018 .018 .018 .018 .018 .018 .018
t 5.65 3.06 2.29 .003 3.05 2.29 4.51
p <.001 .06 .62 1 .06 .62 <.001
LSubLH Est. −.046 −.059 −.1 −.046 −.06 −.02
SE .018 .018 .018 .018 .018 .018
t −2.61 −3.33 −5.66 −2.58 −3.37 −1.11
p .26 .02 <.001 .28 .02 1
LObRH Est. −.013 .054 <−.001 −.014 .026
SE .018 .018 .018 .018 .018
t −.75 3.06 −.01 −.77 1.49
p 1 .06 1 1 1
LSubRH Est. .041 −.013 <−.001 .04
SE .018 .018 .018 .018
t 2.29 −.75 −.02 2.23
p .62 1 1 .72
RObLH Est. .054 .041 .08
SE .018 .018 .018
t 3.05 2.29 4.52
p .06 .62 <.001
RSubLH Est. −.014 .026
SE .018 .018
t −.78 1.47
p 1 1
RObRH Est. .04
SE .018
t 2.25
p .68

2.3 Discussion

The results from Experiment 1 show a penalty associated with left-hand object responses, whereas subject responses fare equally well on either hand. This basic effect appears to also be driving the results of the three-way interaction, in which there was no difference between objects and subjects on the right side and the right hand. Differences emerged when these responses were shifted to the left side and left hand, and this effect seemed to be reduced on the right side with the left hand. The results are thus encouraging, showing that there does indeed appear to be an underlying spatial component to transitive sentences, and that this is modulated by the hand or perhaps response lateralization more generally.

It is, however, also possible that these results are merely an effect of the task. Participants were explicitly asked to map subjects and objects onto either the left or right hand. Manual responses are known to interact with the position of stimuli (Simon, 1969). Thus, it is too premature to draw any strong conclusions from the results of this experiment. In addition, it is crucial to test whether there is still a spatial preference to the entities of transitive structures when they are not explicitly mapped onto alternate hands. At the same time, it is also important to tease apart word order in the sentence so as to better understand this effect. Subjects appear in the sentence before objects, but in our sentences the object was not the final word in the sentence. How would participants fare when responding to the last word in a sentence? Is it simply the case that they are creating a mental map of the word order? We address both the word order and hand mapping problems in Experiment 2.

3 Experiment 2

In E2 we test whether the spatial effect found in E1 can be observed when no direct attention is drawn to the fact that the probe words are the subjects and objects of the previously read sentences. In addition, we compare recognition of subject and object to the final word in the sentence. van Dijck and Fias (2011) reported a SNARC-like effect for items in working memory, such that earlier appearing items in a list have a left-side advantage and later items a right-side advantage. Given the importance of working memory in language processing, it may be the case that the difference we observed between left- and right-side processing of subject and object probe words is merely related to order in the sentence (i.e., the order words are introduced into working memory). If this syntax-space effect is just driven by serial word order, then the final word should show the strongest right-side bias. In this task, then, participants judged whether a probe word was in the sentence they had just read (a go/no-go task). Finding a syntax-space effect using such a task will provide further evidence that language comprehenders naturally generate spatial schemata when encountering transitive sentences and that these schemata follow a predictable pattern with the tendency of subjects to be placed to the left of objects. In addition, not finding an effect of the final word of the sentence will provide evidence that the syntax-space effect is specific for the thematic roles in the sentence and not necessarily just the order words appear in a sentence.

3.1 Methods

3.1.1 Participants

Eighty-one participants (63 female, 18 male, Mage = 20.1) took part in this study for extra credit toward a psychology class. All participants were right-handed (self-reported), native speakers of English with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (self-reported). Participants gave their informed consent to participate in this research under the guidelines of the University of South Carolina Institutional Review Board.

3.1.2 Procedure

The procedure for this experiment is similar to the previous one with a few alterations. The main alteration is that we no longer asked participants to identify the subject and object of the previously read sentence. Instead, they read a sentence, encountered a probe, and judged whether the probe word was in the sentence they had just read. Half of the probe words were either the Subject, Verb, Object, or Final word from the sentence they read (Present), and the other half were probes from another item not used as Present probes within that participant's session (False). Present probes required the participant to press the middle button on the button box. False probes were ignored. Since we increased the number of conditions in this experiment, we doubled the number of items as well. This involved modifying some of the items from Experiment 1 and writing new ones (see Fig. 1 for a sample item in all conditions and Table A2 for all items). We made sure to balance the gender of the characters across items: with one quarter of the items containing all male characters, one quarter all female, one quarter with a male subject and female object, and the final quarter with a female subject and a male object.

The Subjects and Objects in our items were equated for length (Subject = 5.34, range: 3–10; Object = 5.36, range: 3–10; t(159) = 0.23, > .05). No attempt was made to equate the length of the Verbs and Final words with the Subjects and Objects. Final words were an average of 6.29 letters long (range: 3–12), significantly longer than Subjects, t(159) = 4.63, < .001, and Objects, t(159) = 4.67, < .001. We discuss potential implications for this difference (mainly why it is not a concern) in the discussion to this experiment. As we are only interested in comparing the Subject and Object with the Final word, and because all of these are nouns and there are baseline differences in reading times between nouns and verbs, especially across hemispheres (Sereno, 1999), we did not analyze Verb length or any spatial bias in responding to them. Sentences were an average of 6.37 words long (range: 5–7).

In addition to altering the probe recognition task, we also added comprehension questions following one-third of the items. Half of these required a Yes response (i.e., a button press) and the other required a No response (i.e., no button press). The purpose of the comprehension questions was to make sure that participants were not just memorizing the sentences as lists of words and were actually reading to understand the items.

Roughly half of the participants were instructed to respond to probes and comprehension questions using their right hand (= 38), and the rest (= 43) used their left hand. The reason for this manipulation was that the syntax-space effect found in the first experiment seemed to be related to response hand. If this was indeed an essential factor, then only the group responding with the left hand should show a spatial bias in responding to probes. If, however, the effect from the first experiment was a result of a combination of a syntax-space effect with a task dependency effect (for example, an easier mapping of the Subject onto the left hand), then we may still observe a syntax-space effect when the task does not depend on such a word-response hand mapping (see Fig. 1 for a schematic sample trial).

3.2 Results

As we did in E1, before conducting data analyses, we removed all incorrect probe responses from the dataset (3.33%). Next, we removed trials in which participants made incorrect response to the comprehension question (4.39%). One participant was removed entirely due to only achieving around chance on the comprehension questions (57%). Finally, we trimmed out extreme responses faster than 300 ms and slower than 3,000 ms (0.29%). In all, then, 8.01% of responses were removed. For the below analyses we looked at log-transformed response times.

As in E1, data analyses were conducted in the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2011) in R (v.3.1.1; R Development Core Team, 2014), using mixed-effects modeling with stimulus position (Side) with levels Left and Right, between-subjects factor Hand with levels LHand and RHand, and word type (Word) with levels Subject, Object, and Final as fixed effects, as well as all possible interactions between the three factors. Items were treated as random effects, as well as participants, both with the interaction between Word and Subject as random slopes (Baayen et al., 2008). Full model results are reported in Table 3. Statistical analyses followed the same procedure as in E1, starting with the maximum model, and removing coefficient by coefficient to find the model of best fit. The three-way interaction was significant, χ2(2) = 7.12, = .03 (see Fig. 3).

Table 3. Fixed effects for the max model in Experiment 2. Baseline levels are Object, Left Side, and Left Hand
Coefficient estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.694 0.029 230.867 <0.001
β1 Final 0.007 0.025 0.27 ≤0.787
β2 Subject −0.001 0.025 −0.007 ≤0.994
β3 RightSide −0.047 0.025 −1.852 ≤0.064.
β4 RHand 0.072 0.042 1.726 ≤0.086.
β5 Final × RightSide 0.038 0.035 1.091 ≤0.276
β6 Subject × RightSide 0.095 0.036 2.685 ≤0.007
β7 Final × RHand −0.013 0.037 −0.346 ≤0.73
β8 Subject × RHand 0.038 0.037 1.01 ≤0.313
β9 SideRight × RHand 0.027 0.036 0.741 ≤0.459
β10 Final × SideRight × RHand −0.033 0.052 −0.641 ≤0.521
β11 Subject × SideRight × RHand −0.137 0.052 −2.633 ≤0.009

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.
Details are in the caption following the image
Graphical results of Experiment 2, showing the interaction between Side, Hand, and Word. Log-transformed RTs on the y-axis. Bars represent SE.

We explored the three-way interaction by dividing the datasets into the LHand and RHand participant groups, then reran the analyses for each group with just the fixed effects of Word, Side, and the Word by Side interaction. There was an interaction for the LHand group, χ2(2) = 6.96, = .03, but not the RHand group, χ2(2) = 1.56, = .46. Since the interaction was not significant for the RHand group, we further tested the main effects. Word was not significant, χ2(2) = 0.99, = .61, but Side was, χ2(1) = 4.5, = .03, with faster Right Side responses than Left Side.

Regarding the Word by Side interaction on the LHand group, we ran pairwise comparisons using a Bonferroni adjustment. Only one contrast was significant, finding that Right-Side Object responses were faster than Right-Side Subject responses, β = −0.091, SE = 0.023, = −4.025, p < .001. Crucially, there were no differences between responses to Final and Object words on the Right Side, β = −0.043, SE = 0.022, = 1.92, p = .83 (see Table 4 for all contrasts).

Table 4. Pairwise comparisons for the two-way interaction between Side (Left-L, Right-R) and Word (Subject, Object, Final) in E2 for the LHand group, each cell reporting coefficient difference estimates, SE, t ratios, and p values
Condition LObject LSubject RFinal RObject RSubject
LFinal .007 .009 .008 .051 −.04
.022 .022 .022 .022 .022
.31 .412 .385 2.362 −1.811
1 1 1 .276 1
LObject .002 .002 .045 −.047
.023 .023 .023 .023
.091 .067 1.963 −2.032
1 1 .75 .637
LSubject −.001 .043 −.049
.022 .022 .022
−.023 1.905 −2.173
1 .857 .451
RFinal .043 −.048
.022 .023
1.92 −2.134
.828 .497
RObject −.091
.023
−4.025
<.001

3.3 Discussion

In E2 we found a three-way interaction similar to what was found in E1: the syntax-space effect was present in the group responding with their left hand, but not with the right hand group, and additionally the spatial interaction was only apparent for subject and object probe word responses. The findings from this experiment tell us several things. First, the fact that we found a syntax-space effect in a task where subject and object responses were not mapped onto the left and right hands indicates that this effect is robust to variations in the task. It is possible certain types of tasks exaggerate the effect (note, for example, the larger number of significant contrasts in E1 compared to E2), but the syntax-space effect is not merely a product of task. Second, response lateralization does not seem to be a crucial factor as in this experiment responses were made toward a button aligned with the center of the monitor. Third, the sensitivity of the left hand to the syntax-space effect suggests that the effect is related to differences in hemispheric processing, very similar to what Zwaan and Yaxley (2003) reported, Finally, the fact that the effect was strongest for objects and subjects indicates that the syntax-space effect is not merely due to the positioning of words in the sentence (i.e., not a pure working memory order effect, as observed by van Dijck & Fias, 2011) but perhaps that it reflects mental model construction biases, where the thematic roles have clear spatial preferences. The final word, which in our items were typically temporal or locative expressions, would not show any clear bias because they were integral to the entire scenario and could not easily be assigned to a mental space. This finding extends and reinforces the results of E1 in showing the interaction of space, hand, and syntax.

In the Methods section of this experiment we noted that the final words of the sentence were on average longer than both the subjects and objects. In fact, such a difference might have been argued to be problematic. According to the magnitude-space literature, more of something produces rightward biases, whereas less of something produces leftward biases. The difference in length of these words therefore might have led some to predict that final words would indeed lead to faster right-side responses. By the same token, the greater saliency of subjects might lead to the somewhat counterintuitive prediction that they would show a right-side preference over objects (assuming, of course, that saliency can be considered a characteristic with variable magnitude). That we did not see such effects suggests that the SNARC and related effects do not apply to this syntax-space effect if we are just considering issues of magnitude such as word length and degree of saliency.

Of course, position within the sentence is still an issue. The question remains whether or not this is an effect of the word order and left-to-right orthography of English. It is possible that readers (or perhaps language comprehenders in general) assign whatever thematic role they encounter first into the left-side space and the second to the right-side space. In other words, the concept of agency is not the driving factor of the spatial bias. To test this question, we turn now to E3.

4 Experiment 3

Both E1 and E2 found effects of a spatial bias for actors in a transitive sentence, although they differed somewhat in nature, possibly due to the involvement of response laterality. However, E1 suggested a left-hand, left-side penalty for objects, and E2 found that object responses were faster on the right side than the left, showing some cross-experiment concordance. The question remains whether this is an effect of Subject- and Object-hood (i.e., order of mention) or Agent- and Patient-hood (i.e., role in the transitive sentence). The most obvious way to test this is to see whether passivization of transitive sentences reverses the effect we have so far observed. If it does lead to agents now being responded to slower than patients on the left side and/or with the left hand, we can conclude that any relation between syntax and space is related to order of mention (subject before object) and not the underlying causal structure of the event. On the other hand, if there is no reversal (agents are still responded to faster than patients on the left side and/or with the left hand despite being the grammatical objects of the passive sentences), we can conclude that transitive sentences evoke a kind of action space with placeholders for agents and patients. Thus, in E3 we add a further level of complexity to the design: sentence voice. In addition, we reintroduce response laterality into the design (the reason for doing this is described in the Methods section).

4.1 Methods

4.1.1 Participants

Sixty-six participants (49 female, 17 male, Mage = 20.6) took part in this study for extra credit toward a psychology class. All participants were right-handed (self-reported), native speakers of English with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (self-reported). Participants gave their informed consent to participate in this research under the guidelines of the University of South Carolina Institutional Review Board.

4.1.2 Procedure

In addition to testing the question of whether voice alters the syntax-space effect, we modified the methods of the previous experiments to resolve some problems. We used the 160 items from E2 and created a full passive version of each item (i.e., with a by-phrase introducing the agent). Some items needed to be altered further because they sounded awkward when changed to the passive voice. Some names were changed as well. As we did before, length of Agent and Patient names were equated (Agent = 5.37, range: 3–9; Object = 5.35, range: 3–10; t(159) = 0.29, = .77). Gender of Agents and Patients remained balanced across items. We did not attempt to equate the length of Active and Passive sentences (i.e., we did not add extra words to Active sentences or remove them from Passive sentences). Active sentences were 6.44 words long on average (range: 5–7), whereas Passive sentences were 8.44 words long on average (range: 7–9). This difference in length, given the complications of the relation between magnitude and space, is potentially problematic for our task. We will address this issue further below.

In addition to these modifications to the sentences, we removed the go/no-go aspect of the design and reintroduced response laterality. This allowed us to collect data for False probes (which we were not able to do in E2), as a useful comparison with the Agent and Patient probe responses. The participants' task was to read the sentence (presented word-by-word in the center of the screen as before) and then respond to a probe word presented on the Left or Right side of the computer monitor that was either Present in the sentence they had just read or was pulled from a list of unused names (False). Responses were made on the left- or right-side buttons of a Serial Response Box with the index fingers of the LHand or RHand, respectively. Half of the trials included a False probe and the other half a Present probe. Following each probe was a yes/no comprehension question presented either in the active or passive voice. Voice of the comprehension question was crossed with voice of the item, encouraging participants to understand who was doing what to whom in the sentences. Yes/no button assignment corresponded to Present/False probe responses. After 80 items, button assignment switched, with order of button assignment counterbalanced across participants. This design resulted in the following conditions of interest: stimulus presentation Side (Left, Right), response Hand (LHand, RHand), sentence Voice (Active, Passive), and probe Word (Agent, Patient). Note that in this experiment we refer to agents and patients as opposed to subjects and objects. Recall that the agent is the character in the sentence that initiates the action and the patient is the object or goal of that action. Thus, in this case a word order effect would be reflected in an interaction between Word, Side, and Voice, whereas a causal order effect would be reflected in a Word and Side interaction. Participants were given 10 practice items before each experimental block to grow accustomed to the button assignment. They were not allowed to advance into the actual experiment until they had achieved over 60% accuracy (counting both probe and comprehension question responses).

A final change to this experiment was the introduction of a chinrest to guarantee that the participants' heads remained centered relative to the screen (see Fig. 1 for a schematic sample trial and Table A3 for all items).

4.2 Results

Following the same procedures from E1 and E2, we removed all trials with incorrect probe responses (6.16%), those with incorrect responses to comprehension questions (17.09%), and those with extreme responses (0.32%), a total of 23.57% of trials. In addition, we removed five participants' data due to chance performance (= 54%) on the comprehension questions. Statistical analyses followed the same procedure as in previous experiments, starting with the maximum model, and removing coefficient by coefficient to find the model of best fit (see Table 5 for the maximum model). It was found that the four-way interaction was unnecessary, χ2(1) = 1.6, = .64. We next proceeded to remove those coefficients that were most theoretically interesting to this study. Removing the three-way interaction between Side, Voice, and Word resulted in a significant reduction in variance accounted for, χ2(1) = 4.41, = .04 (see Fig. 4). Next we removed the Word, Hand, and Voice interaction. This too explained a significant amount of variance, χ2(1) = 3.98, = .05 (see Fig. 5). Removing other three-way interactions did not result in any significant changes: removing the Side, Word, and Hand interaction, χ2(1) = 1.69, = .19; removing the Side, Hand, and Voice interaction, χ2(1) < 1.

Table 5. Fixed effects for the max model in Experiment 3. Baseline levels are Agent, Left Side, Left Hand, and Active Voice
Coefficient Estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.909 0.025 277.731 <0.001
β1 RightSide 0.03 0.021 1.377 ≤0.169
β2 Patient 0.107 0.021 5.023 <0.001
β3 RHand −0.021 0.022 −0.929 ≤0.353
β4 Passive 0.097 0.022 4.469 <0.001
β5 RightSide × Patient −0.05 0.028 −1.67 ≤0.095.
β6 RightSide × RHand −0.072 0.03 −2.347 ≤0.019
β7 Patient × RHand −0.081 0.03 −2.631 ≤0.009
β8 RightSide × Passive −0.02 0.031 −0.652 ≤0.515
β9 Patient × Passive −0.224 0.03 −7.437 <0.001
β10 RHand × Passive −0.021 0.031 −0.673 ≤0.501
β11 RightSide × Patient × RHand 0.054 0.043 1.257 ≤0.209
β12 RightSide × Patient × Passive 0.077 0.042 1.824 ≤0.068.
β13 RightSide × RHand × Passive 0.019 0.043 0.443 ≤0.658
β14 Patient × RHand × Passive 0.074 0.043 1.734 ≤0.083.
β15 RightSide × Patient × RHand × Passive −0.028 0.06 −0.466 ≤0.641

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.
Details are in the caption following the image
Graphical results of Experiment 3, showing the interaction between Word, Side, and Voice. Log-transformed RTs on the y-axis. Bars represent SE.
Details are in the caption following the image
Graphical results of Experiment 3, showing the interaction between Word, Hand, and Voice. Log-transformed RTs on the y-axis. Bars represent SE.

Next, we analyzed the interaction effects using pairwise comparisons. For each interaction we report adjusted p values using the Bonferroni method, reporting only the theoretically significant results in the text and the rest of the comparisons in the respective tables. Regarding the interaction between Voice, Word, and Side, there was a significant advantage for responses to Agents over responses to Patients on the Left in the Active Voice, β = −0.067, SE = 0.015, = −4.42, p < .001, but this difference was not significant on the Right in the Active Voice, β = −0.044, SE = 0.015, = −2.9, p = .11. In the Passive Voice, responses to Patients were faster than those to Agents on both the Left, β = 0.13, SE = 0.015, = 7.84, p < .001, and Right, β = 0.079, SE = 0.015, = 5.25, p < .001 (see Table 6 for all contrasts).

Table 6. Pairwise comparisons for the three-way interaction between Side (Left-L, Right-R), Word (Agent-A, Patient-P), and Voice (Active-Act, Passive-Pass) in E3, each cell reporting coefficient difference estimates, SE, t ratios, and p values
Condition Value RAAct LPAct RPAct LAPass RAPass LPPass RPPass
LAAct Est. .006 −.067 −.037 −.086 −.07 .033 .01
SE .015 .015 .015 .015 .015 .015 .015
t .41 −4.42 −2.45 −5.59 −4.57 2.18 .63
p 1 <.001 .4 <.001 <.001 .82 1
RAAct Est. −.073 −.044 −.092 −.076 .027 .004
SE .015 .015 .015 .015 .015 .015
t −4.89 −2.9 −6.07 −5.04 1.8 .23
p <.001 .11 <.001 <.001 1 1
LPAct Est. .03 −.019 −.003 .1 .077
SE .015 .015 .015 .015 .015
t 1.98 −1.28 −.2 6.67 5.08
p 1 1 1 <.001 <.001
RPAct Est. −.049 −.032 .071 .047
SE .015 .015 .015 .015
t −3.21 −2.15 4.69 3.1
p .04 .88 <.001 .05
LAPass Est. .017 .12 .096
SE .015 .015 .015
t 1.08 7.84 6.25
p 1 <.001 <.001
RAPass Est. .103 .079
SE .015 .015
t 6.83 5.25
p <.001 <.001
LPPass Est. −.024
SE .015
t −1.56
p 1

Now, we turn to the other significant three-way interaction, Voice, Word, and Hand. Focusing first on the Active Voice, there was an advantage for responding to Agents over Patients on the LHand, β = −0.082, SE = 0.015, = −5.39, p < .001, but no such advantage for the RHand, β = −0.028, SE = 0.015, = −1.82, = 1. For the Passive Voice, there was an advantage for responses to Patients over Agents on both the LHand, β = 0.103, SE = 0.015, = 6.7, p < .001, and the RHand, β = 0.096, SE = 0.016, = 6.18, p < .001 (see Table 7 for all contrasts).

Table 7. Pairwise comparisons for the three-way interaction between Hand (Right Hand-RH, Left Hand-LH), Word (Agent-A, Patient-P), and Voice (Active-Act, Passive-Pass) in E3, each cell reporting coefficient difference estimates, SE, t ratios, and p values
Condition Value PassALH ActPLH PassPLH ActARH PassARH ActPRH PassPRH
ActALH Est. −.086 −.082 .016 .056 −.019 .028 .077
SE .015 .015 .015 .016 .016 .015 .016
t −5.68 −5.39 1.05 3.59 −1.2 1.84 4.98
p <.001 <.001 1 .01 1 1 <.001
PassALH Est. .004 .103 .143 .068 .115 .164
SE .015 .015 .016 .016 .015 .016
t .28 6.7 9.08 .43 7.47 10.55
p 1 <.001 <.001 <.001 <.001 <.001
ActPLH Est. .098 .139 .063 .111 .16
SE .015 .015 .015 .015 .015
t 6.6 9.06 4.14 7.16 10.22
p <.001 <.001 .001 <.001 <.001
PassPLH Est. .04 −.035 .012 .061
SE .015 .015 .016 .016
t 2.62 −2.27 .79 3.91
p .25 .65 1 .003
ActARH Est. −.075 −.028 .021
SE .015 .015 .016
t −4.91 −1.82 1.35
p <.001 1 1
PassARH Est. .047 .096
SE .015 .016
t 3.06 6.18
p .06 <.001
ActPRH Est. .049
SE .015
t 3.22
p .04

Finally, since this experiment involved responding to False probes, we analyzed these separately to test the possibility that responding to probes unassociated with any linguistic context will also interact with manual or spatial processes. For this analysis the maximal model included the variables Hand, Side, and Voice, with participants and items as random factors with maximal random slope structure. As before we proceeded to remove interaction effects one by one, comparing the change in variance with the previous model (see Table 8). The only significant effect was a main effect of Hand, with RHand responses slower than LHand, χ2(1) = 11.85, < .001, likely indicating participants' preference to respond with their right hand to indicate correct responses and their left hand to indicate incorrect ones.

Table 8. Fixed effects for the max model of False probe responses in Experiment 3. Baseline levels are Left Hand, Left Side, and Active Voice
Coefficient Estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.866 0.023 294.224 <0.001
β1 RightSide −0.025 0.014 −1.783 ≤0.075.
β2 RHand 0.013 0.014 0.918 ≤0.359
β3 Passive −0.0002 0.014 −0.02 ≤0.984
β4 RightSide × RHand 0.02 0.02 0.995 ≤0.32
β5 RightSide × Passive 0.015 0.02 0.755 ≤0.451
β6 RHand × Passive 0.007 0.02 0.329 ≤0.742
β7 RightSide × RHand × Passive 0.001 0.028 0.046 ≤0.963

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.

4.3 Discussion

In E3 we aimed to test the impact of sentence voice on the syntax-space effect we have so far observed in our experiments. We also wondered how this effect would interact with the hand, namely response laterality.

First, regarding the three-way interaction between stimulus side, sentence voice, and probe word, a simple trend is apparent in Fig. 4. Following active sentences, patients suffer a left-side response penalty, leading to a difference between agent and patient responses. However, no such penalty occurs on the right side. Agents do not appear to elicit a response advantage on either side. For the passive voice, patients are responded to just as quickly on the left as on the right, faster on both sides than the by-phrase agent. Thus, it seems the spatial effect is present in active sentences and becomes reduced in passive sentences. While this is not a simple case of agents always showing a left-side bias, importantly, the results following passive sentences are not a mirror image of the active sentence results. This shows that the causal structure of the sentences plays a role in these effects and, furthermore, that these effects cannot be attributed solely to differences between grammatical subjects and grammatical objects and their relative order in the sentence.

Perhaps related to this three-way interaction between word, voice, and side is the other three-way interaction between response hand, voice, and word. Inspection of Fig. 5 shows nearly the same effect that we just described, except now for hands as opposed to space. For the active voice there is an effect on the left hand, with agents faster than patients, but this effect disappears on the right hand. For the passive voice the difference in salience between agents and patients remains constant across hand. This further reinforces the role of the causal structure in these effects.

Finally and importantly, responding to the false probes in this experiment did not lead to any interactions between voice, hand, or space. An interesting and unexpected finding was that, counterintuitively, when examining the responses to false probes, there was a benefit on the left hand as opposed to the right hand. A simple explanation for this is that participants prefer to use their right hand for correct responses and their left hand for incorrect ones. This is perhaps in line with the MARC effect reported by Nuerk et al. (2004).

Overall, while the effects of this experiment are suggestive about a spatial representation of thematic roles in a syntactic frame, we must wonder again, especially given the differences we have observed across experiments (i.e., the syntax-space effect in this experiment was not only present on the left hand as it was in E1 and E2), to what extent does this effect depend separately on hand and space? We address this issue in the final experiment.

5 Experiment 4

So far we have provided evidence for a syntax-space effect (E1), which is still present when response laterality is removed (E2) but is nevertheless only present in active sentences, suggesting that the saliency of the agent drives the effect (E2 and E3). While response laterality was removed in E2, responses were still made manually. This final experiment, therefore, asks whether this effect remains when participants must make vocal responses using a design similar to what was used in E3. We hypothesize that, given the previously observed hand interactions, indeed the syntax-space effect is intimately connected with the laterality of the effector, and thus no effect will be present in E4.

5.1 Methods

5.1.1 Participants

Forty-one participants (29 female, 12 male, Mage = 20.93) took part in this study for extra credit toward a psychology class. All participants were right-handed (self-reported) and were native speakers of English with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (self-reported). Participants gave their informed consent to participate in this research under the guidelines of the University of South Carolina Institutional Review Board.

5.1.2 Procedure

The procedure was identical to that of E3 with two modifications. First, responses were made using a microphone connected to a Serial Response Box, which was used for measuring response times, as well as another microphone that recorded the actual responses. Microphones were placed side by side and angled toward the participant to maximize reception. Participants were instructed to say “yes” to probes that were in the previous sentence and “no” to probes that were not and similarly to answers of the yes/no comprehension questions. The second change to the procedure was that there was no longer a second practice after the first experimental block. The reason for including this practice in E3 was to allow the participant to acclimatize to the new button-response mapping, and as the present experiment used vocal responses, this was no longer necessary. Thus, in this experiment the factors of interest were Voice (Active, Passive), Word (Agent, Patient), and Side (Left, Right).

5.2 Results

Following the same procedures used previously, we removed all trials with incorrect probe responses (6.25%), incorrect responses to the comprehension question (19.05%), and those with extreme responses (2.87%), a total of 28.17% of trials. In addition, we removed seven participants' data due to chance performance (= 46%) on the comprehension questions. Note that questions in E3 and E4 varied between active and passive voice. Thus, half of the questions appeared in the opposite voice to the one presented in the item (e.g., a passive voiced question following an active voiced item). This may account for the relatively low accuracy on the comprehension questions in these two experiments compared to in E2. Log-transformed response times to probes were analyzed.

Statistical analyses followed the same procedure as in previous experiments, starting with the maximum model, and removing coefficient by coefficient to find the model of best fit (see Table 9 for the maximum model). It was found that the three-way interaction between Voice, Word, and Side was unnecessary, χ2(1) = 0.65, = .42, so we proceeded to remove the two-way interaction coefficients from the model (see Fig. 6). The Side by Word interaction did not account for a significant amount of variance, χ2(1) = 0.05, = .82. However, removing the Word by Voice interaction did account for a significant amount of variance, χ2(1) = 24.23, < .001. The Side and Voice interaction was also unnecessary, χ2(1) = 0.96, = .33.

Table 9. Fixed effects for the max model in Experiment 4. Baseline levels are Agent, Left Side, and Active Voice
Coefficient Estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.933 0.029 236.326 <0.001
β1 RightSide −0.0001 0.017 −0.004 ≤0.997
β2 Patient 0.045 0.017 2.582 ≤0.01
β3 Passive 0.042 0.018 2.415 ≤0.016
β4 RightSide × Patient −0.018 0.025 −0.731 ≤0.465
β5 RightSide × Passive 0.002 0.025 0.098 ≤0.922
β6 Patient × Passive −0.1 0.025 −4.052 <0.001
β7 RightSide × Patient × Passive 0.028 0.035 0.808 ≤0.419

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.
Details are in the caption following the image
Graphical results of Experiment 4, showing the non-significant interaction between Word, Side, and Voice. Log-transformed RTs on the y-axis. Bars represent SE.

Next, we present post hoc comparisons of the interaction between Word and Voice, using the appropriate Bonferroni method of p value adjustment. Responses to Active Agents were on average faster than those to Passive Agents, β = −0.044, SE = 0.012, = −3.52, = .003, and Active Patients, β = −0.036, SE = 0.012, = −2.92, = .02, whereas responses to Passive Patients were faster than those to Passive Agents, β = 0.05, SE = 0.012, = 4.07, p < .001, and Active Patients, β = 0.042, SE = 0.012, = 3.45, p < .001 (see Table 10 for all contrasts).

Table 10. Pairwise comparisons for the two-way interaction between Word (Agent-Ag, Patient-Pat) and Voice (Active-Act, Passive-Pass) in E4, each cell reporting coefficient difference estimates, SE, t ratios, and p values
Condition Value PassAg ActPat PassPat
ActAg Est. −.044 −.036 .006
SE .012 .012 .012
t −3.52 −2.92 .512
p .003 .02 1
PassAg Est. −.008 .05
SE .012 .012
t −.62 4.07
p 1 <.001
ActPat Est. .042
SE .012
t 3.45
p <.001

As we did in E3, we analyzed the False probe responses only, using similar analyses as with the Present probe responses. The maximal model included the factors Side and Voice and the interaction, and participants and items as random factors with maximal random slope structure. None of the effects were statistically significant (see Table 11 for the maximal model).

Table 11. Fixed effects for the max model of False Probe responses in Experiment 4. Baseline levels are Left Side and Active Voice
Coefficient Estimates
β Condition Est. SE t p
β0 (Intercept) 6.977 0.027 262.253 <0.001
β1 RightSide −0.011 0.012 −0.92 ≤0.358
β2 Passive 0.004 0.012 0.356 ≤0.722
β3 RightSide × Passive 0.019 0.017 1.113 ≤0.266

Note

  • ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05; borderline effects p < .1 and >.05.

5.3 Discussion

E4 failed to find a syntax-space effect. It did, however, find an interaction between probe word and sentence voice. This is somewhat similar to Ferreira's (2003) finding of greater difficulty in processing passives than actives, as well as the patient being more accessible in memory following passive than active sentences. The lack of the syntax-space effect was expected and inspection of Table 9 shows that this was not a case of a lack of power, as there was not even a trend in the expected direction. Given that we failed to find a spatial interaction in this experiment, it seems likely that the effects observed in the rest of the experiments are related either to the specific manual motor effector or the lateralization of responses (or a combination of the two). We explore this idea further in the General Discussion.

6 General discussion

The goal of this study has been to investigate the possibility that the actors in transitive sentences come with lateral spatial biases, as follows from claims that the syntactic frame of sentences is spatial in nature (e.g., Chatterjee, 2001; Talmy, 2000). We tested this idea using a novel technique inspired by tasks used in the magnitude-space and Simon effect literature (e.g., Dehaene et al., 1993; Gevers et al., 2003; Rubichi & Nicoletti, 2006). This new design may prove to be useful in the future for researchers interested in further exploring the relationship between language, space, and hand. E1 had participants read transitive sentences and judge whether probe words presented to the left and right side of the computer monitor were the subjects or objects of the sentences they had read, making lateralized button presses. E2 employed a go/no-go task, with participants responding to subjects, objects, and the final words of the sentences with a central button press. E3 combined strengths of the designs from E1 and E2 with the introduction of the variable of sentence voice. Finally, E4 removed button pressing from the design, repeating E3 with vocal responses. All experiments except E4 found similar interactions among the variables of word and space. The involvement of the hand indicated stronger syntax-space effects either when responding toward the left side of space or when right hemisphere motor regions were engaged (Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003) and perhaps greater involvement of more dorsal visual pathways (Gozli et al., 2012). Specifically, E1 found a penalty associated with left-side object responses made with the left hand. The contributions of the hands in this experiment may have reflected a preference for assigning the subject to the left hand and the object to the right, potentially because this hand-word assignment mirrors the proposed syntactic spatial frame. However, E2 also found a spatial effect dependent upon response hand, showing that the spatial effect associated with subjects and objects does not depend on response laterality but the specific hand responding. Results also showed the final words show no facilitation or penalty on either side. E3 found that voice and word interacted separately with space and hand. Both of these interactions can be described as showing spatial effects for the active voice, such that patients have a left-side penalty. E4 only found a word-voice interaction. It should be pointed out that while the interaction between word and voice observed in E4 was not the main interest of this study, it does verify the validity and reliability of the paradigm we have developed. If in using this design we were able to replicate well-established psycholinguistic findings (e.g., Ferreira, 2003), the design is probably also suitable to detect other non-canonical effects (see Table 12 for summary of findings). We consider the implications of all of these findings below.

Table 12. Summary of results from all experiments
Experiment Task Variables Results
E1 Grammatical role Word, Side, Hand Word × Side × Hand
E2 Go/no-go Word, Side, Hand Word × Side × Hand
E3 Present/absent Word, Side, Hand, Voice Word × Side × Voice
Word × Hand × Voice
E4 Present/absent Word, Side, Voice Word × Voice

First, to get a better understanding of what the findings from these experiments can tell us about the relation between transitivity, space, and hand, let us break down the task of comprehending a transitive sentence. From a strictly formalistic syntactic view, there are two distinct thematic roles, and there is an action originating from one (the subject or agent) and terminating at the other (the object or patient). Knowledge of the event crucially depends on understanding how it unfolds in time. Langacker (2001) argued that when word order corresponds with conceptual order the sentence should be easier to understand, and indeed, difficulty in understanding passive sentences may be a result of the formation of shallow representations, based on these conflicting orders (Ferreira, Bailey, & Ferraro, 2002). Difficulty with passives may also involve the movement of syntactic elements within the sentence's deep structure to their position in the surface structure (Chomsky, 1957). The idea of conflicting temporal representations (e.g., between the fleeting memory of words in the sentence and the imagery formed of the event) might result in a neutral spatial preference. On the other hand, when the two factors align, a clear preference is formed for agents on the left and patients on the right. This was the case in E3, in which the passive voice resulted in no spatial bias, whereas the active voice did result in such a bias. The spatial bias fits in with those observed in previous research looking at scene production and recognition (e.g., Chatterjee, 2001; Chokron & De Agostini, 2000; Maas & Russo, 2003) and potentially even with the findings from the magnitude-space literature (e.g., Bueti & Walsh, 2009; Dehaene et al., 1993), all of which seems to be related to orthographic directionality (although speakers of languages with right-to-left orthographies will have to be tested using our paradigm in the future to confirm this intuition). However, it should be noted that orthography may not be the driving factor here. de Hevia et al. (2014) showed preverbal infants prefer magnitudes increasing from left-to-right over right-to-left, and Rugani et al. (2015) reported that the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) also shows evidence for a left-to-right mental number line. In addition, Maas and Russo (2003) found that orthographic direction seems to have a combinatorial effect with the overall preference to process actions as moving from left-to-right, exaggerating the effect in speakers of Romance languages and reducing it in Arabic speakers. In sum, agents are leftward and patients rightward due to the order within which they canonically occur, both superficially and conceptually. Crucially, we show that this generalization holds for a wide range of verbs (E2 and E3 used a total of 160 different transitive verbs) varying in imageability. However, when a noncanonical order is used, there is no preference, and given the various bodies of evidence just reviewed, we would not expect to find one. In addition, this syntax-space effect only seems to emerge when there is a direct interaction with space (via the hands in our experiments).

It is intriguing to note that the spatial effect seems to disappear (or is at least reduced) on the right (both hand and side). This can be explained in at least a couple of ways. One possible explanation involves language laterality and is based on the assumption that the spatial representations evoked by transitive sentences in right-handed people are neurally right lateralized. When we respond with our right hands (or vocally), we are involving left hemispheric motor regions to a greater extent than right hemispheric ones. In this case, the spatial representation that was formed is not directly accessed. However, when left-hand responses are made, the spatial representation facilitates (or interferes with) responding, based simply on the priming of these structures. In line with this explanation are reports that hemispatial neglect, characterized by a deficit in attention to the left side of space, typically results from damage to the right parietal and temporal cortices (Karnath & Rorden, 2012), homologs of left-hemispheric regions implicated in language processing and more specifically in the processing of transitive syntactic frames (Den Ouden, Fix, Parrish, & Thompson, 2009; Shetreet, Palti, Friedmann, & Hadar, 2007; Thompson et al., 2007). Furthermore, the motor cortex engages regions in the parietal lobes involved with spatial planning (Colby & Goldberg, 1999). This explanation is also in line with the findings of Zwaan and Yaxley (2003), who reported that the canonical positioning of stimuli impacted semantic judgment response times only when the stimuli were presented to the right hemisphere (via the left visual field). Under this explanation, the non-linguistic right-hemisphere homologs, interacting with right-hemisphere motor regions, take a supporting role in processing the spatial-syntactic frame through direct activation from the linguistic stimuli. Also, not necessarily alternative to the above explanations, but possibly playing a contributing role are the observations that the involvement of the hands alters how stimuli are processed (Chan et al., 2013; Gozli et al., 2012). Specifically, when stimuli are processed in near-hand space, there is greater contribution of dorsal visual pathways. All of these various studies taken together point toward the involvement of right parietal structures via left-handed responses in contributing to the syntax-space effect.

Of course, it is also possible that using the hands generally enhances spatial attention and that this attention is flexible, depending on such factors as the positioning of the hands in space. If this is indeed the case, the various hand-related effects reported in E1 and E3 were not actually hand effects, but rather were spatial effects in the sense that responding required interacting with discrete locations in space. Indeed, in the experiments in which responses were lateralized we found the strongest results (recall the small number of significant pairwise comparisons in E2). This would suggest that perhaps it is not necessarily the hands that are important for eliciting this effect. Instead, it is having a spatial dimension in the response (no matter what the motor effector) that is really crucial. According to this explanation, using different fingers of one hand to press lateralized buttons, using the feet to press pedals, or even nodding toward different locations may produce similar effects. The fact that SNARC effects have been elicited when using pedal responses lends some support to this notion (Schwarz & Müller, 2006). That there may be separate contributions of neural and response lateralization suggests the need for a more careful manipulation of the positioning of the hands as well as comparisons with other motor effectors. Indeed, studies looking at SNARC (Wood et al., 2006) and STEARC (Vallesi, Binns, & Shallice, 2008) have found that crossing the hands eliminates the respective effects, indicating a role for both hand and space. Similarly, Rubichi and Nicoletti (2006), finding larger Simon effects in the dominant-hand controlled hemifield, showed that crossing hands shifts the effect over to the opposite hemifield. This proposal of course would raise innumerable possible variations on designs for testing the nature of this syntax-space effect.

Finally, both of these suggestions may be partially correct, as we have reported in these experiments that while all three elements of language, hand, and space interact, they do not do so in a very straightforward way. For example, E3 found two separate three-way interactions, which suggests that both the hand and space contribute to language processing separately. Indeed, it has been suggested that praxis and language overlap to some degree, both conceptually and neuroanatomically (Kobayashi & Ugawa, 2013); the hands provide a ready tool for the deaf in signing, a language as complex as any spoken language (Emmorey, 1999); manual gesturing facilitates speech production (Rauscher, Krauss, & Chen, 1996); it has been suggested that the human capacity for language evolved out of a gestural protolanguage (Corballis, 2010; Rizzolatti & Arbib, 1998); at an ontological level, gestural babbling precedes vocal babbling in the human infant (Petitto & Marentette, 1991; Volterra & Iverson, 1995); and (gesturing aside) the importance of the hands in language has found a resurgence in such recent technological advances as texting and online chatting, the immediacy of which emulate that of spoken conversation (Corballis, 2011).

The relation between the hands and spatial processing seems noncontroversial. Use of the hands guides our understanding of space, though certainly one can lack hands and still have a concept of space. Similarly, while some researchers have noted the importance of the hands even in vocal speech production (Rauscher et al., 1996), it is clear also that language can and does exist without the hands. The connection of language and space is perhaps more speculative, though even this area has its proponents (e.g., Chatterjee, 2001; Croft, 1991; Langacker, 2001; Talmy, 2000), and indeed the rare co-occurrence of aphasia and neglect has been noted following left-hemispheric insults (Suchan & Karnath, 2011). Reading is clearly a visuospatial task, and its effects on development are of critical importance. However, speakers from non-literate cultures may not show any connections between syntax and space when tested empirically, and Arabic or Hebrew speakers may show the reverse of the effects we have shown here (or perhaps a right-side agent/left-side patient advantage on the left hand only). Evidence from both populations will be necessary to further elucidate the syntax-space effect. In addition, given the importance of semantics in spatial processing (e.g., Bergen, Lindsay, Matlock, & Narayanan, 2010; Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003), there are likely to be some differences between the spatial effect elicited by different transitive verbs, whether additive or interactive. For example, verbs producing different causal inferences (i.e., implicit causality) might show reversed or reduced syntax-space effects, as well as action verbs denoting action flowing in opposite directions (e.g., push/pull as per Chatterjee et al., 1999; Maas & Russo, 2003). The results of these experiments thus leave us to consider the three distinct yet intertwining (and perhaps overlapping) systems of language, space, and hand.

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by grants NIH R21AG030445, NSF BCS0822617, and a SPARC award from the University of South Carolina Graduate School. We thank the following members of the Language and Cognition aLab for their help in preparing stimuli and running participants: Karen Shebuski, Kristen Bertram, Alan Peh, Amanda Bennett, Trey Gordner, Sara Peters, Kat Wilson, and Evgenia Borschevskaya. Thanks also to Wei Cheng, Rutvik Desai, Jessica Green, Troy Herter, Christiana Keinath, and Shinichi Shoji for their comments on the manuscript.

    Notes

  1. 1 Markedness indicates more basic linguistic forms. Given a pair of words, the less marked form is the one that is more frequent or can be negated without resulting in awkwardness (e.g., un-even vs. un-odd). See Nuerk et al. (2004) for a more complete discussion, or Zimmer (1964) and Lyons (1969).
  2. 2 Participant number was based on a pilot study not reported here with a very similar design to the experiments run in this study. Variations of this number in later experiments accommodate the increased number of factors or levels of factors to maintain consistent power.
  3. 3 The exact model is as follows: log(RT) ~ Hand × Word × Side + (1 × Word × Hand × Side¦Subject) + (1 × Word × Hand × Side¦Item).
  4. 4 Note it is not recommended to interpret model p values directly. There are several different ways to make statistical inferences about the fit of linear mixed-effects models, for example, likelihood ratio testing or model selection. The interested reader should consult Bolker et al. (2009) or Pinheiro and Bates (2000) for a discussion of the benefits and drawbacks of different methods.
  5. 5 The analyses were performed in two different ways, based on comments from an anonymous reviewer. In one analysis we removed trials with incorrect responses to questions and in the other we did not. Indeed, this led to a difference in the results, with the former producing a three-way interaction and the latter two different two-way interactions (Hand × Word, Side × Word). This suggests that, indeed, comprehension is a critical factor in the syntax-space effect. For brevity, we only report details of the three-way interaction in this article.
  6. 6 The full model: log(RT) ~ Word × Side × Hand + (1 × Word × Side|Subject) + (1 × Word × Side|Item).
  7. 7 The full model: log(RT) ~ Word × Hand × Side × Voice + (1 × Word × Hand × Side × Voice|Subject) + (1 × Word × Hand × Side × Voice|Item).
  8. 8 The full model: log(RT) ~ Hand × Side × Voice + (1 × Hand × Side × Voice|Subject) + (1 × Hand × Side × Voice|Item).
  9. 9 The full model: log(RT) ~ Voice × Word × Side + (1 × Voice × Word × Side|Subject) + (1 × Voice × Word × Side|Item).
  10. 10 The full model: log(RT) ~ Voice × Side + (1 × Voice × Side|Subject) + (1 × Voice × Side|Item).
  11. Appendix A

    Table A1. Items From Experiment 1
    Wendy thanked Janet after the meal
    Brandy presented Samantha at the gala reception
    Carmella thought about Liza during the war
    Joan avoided Susan for the entire semester
    Jill met Brenda at the elementary school
    Sarah entertained Michelle during spring break
    Danielle adopted Maggie at the municipal court
    Penelope examined Cindy at the dental clinic
    Nicole slapped Valerie during the rehearsal
    Dawn met Stacy on the dance floor
    Brittany annoyed Catherine during biology class
    Julie warned Andrea in the hotel lobby
    Jessica spotted Holly in the weight room
    Stephanie conversed with Alyssa at the conference
    Rachel arrested Marie in the parking lot
    Laura complimented Sandra at the end of the day
    Steve carried Ben down the steep mountainside
    Thomas saw Bill in the checkout lane
    Derek insulted Brad in the shopping mall
    Phil questioned Kyle during the oral exam
    Christopher high-fived Trent after the fight
    Eddie followed Stan from a safe distance
    Troy found Nathan at the movie store
    Fred shoved Ernie at the top of the slide
    Josh called Robert from across the room
    Ethan took Brian to the new theme park
    Andrew abandoned George in the middle of the date
    Ken passed Richard on the way to work
    Abe left Russel at the end of the evening
    Roger photographed Jeff during the wedding reception
    Frank assisted Brandon before bedtime
    Robbie paid Craig once a month
    Dale hired Greg during the winter holiday
    Kirk caught Raymond behind the curtains
    Jane visited Annie in the recovery room
    Emily raised Kate in the small apartment
    Eden recruited Sally for the basketball team
    Beth rejected Kristin after the big argument
    Alexandra pushed Sophia into the cold water
    Alisha fed Amanda with a small teaspoon
    Whitney noticed Krissy at the holiday party
    Marcia brought Amy to the job fair
    Rita invited Joanna to the bridal shower
    Becca positioned Mary next to the statue
    Rebecca recognized Vivian at the high school reunion
    Margaret flattered Suzanne during the dinner party
    Heather contacted Crystal last summer
    Alexis encouraged Betsy throughout the years
    Liz identified Charlotte at the hospital
    Phoebe escorted Katie around the compound
    Jacquelyn greeted Christine outside the movie theater
    Shawn helped William in the empty house
    Adam criticized Harold during the board meeting
    Ryan scolded Henry after the birthday party
    Desmond praised Tom during the long speech
    Nick watched Carl at the train station
    Jacob inspired Neil during the performance
    Martin applauded James at the end of the play
    Bob stopped Matthew in the waiting room
    Eric bowed to Carlos at the end of the show
    Marcus observed Kenneth at the sales event
    Joseph approached Mike in the dining room
    Ted remembered Drew during art class
    Matt interrogated Zack at the police station
    Anthony advised Theodore during the trial
    Table A2. Items From Experiment 2
    Miranda informed Dominic about the concert
    Bailey electrocuted Marcia with the taser
    Kurt located Gale at the wedding ceremony
    Faith taunted Bill with harsh words
    Elizabeth dismissed Alexandra with a warning
    Fredrick belittled Carolina at the business assembly
    Jason described Connor in the article
    David pursued Nora during the fall term
    Grace motivated Andrew during the workout
    Jared prepared Maria for the job interview
    Nadine reassured Darryl after the presentation
    Donald spied Rachel with the binoculars
    Trent welcomed Chase into the new house
    Austin encountered Shelby at the fresh market
    Seth seated Ella next to the television
    Isabelle saved Penelope on Christmas Day
    Sharon banned Marcus from the lecture hall
    Adam astonished Owen during the function
    Mona startled Roger from behind the door
    Cole cornered June under the tree
    Rebecca acknowledged Carmella with a smile
    Chad amazed Max with the strange tale
    Lyle chased Kayla with a bat
    Sarah bothered Luke with a few complaints
    Thelma collected Amber from the station
    Bryce struck Anna with a ski pole
    Beck corrected Hugh during the pop quiz
    Wade accused Kat during the trial
    Carl found Pete with the tracking device
    Nathan served Ashley at the diner
    Carly disregarded Kevin during the funeral
    Blake included Chuck in the acceptance speech
    Eugene intimidated Violet before the soccer championship
    Beatrice hit Samantha with a ruler
    Johnny woke Melissa before work
    Claire disturbed Tyrone during the big match
    Selena ogled Maggie at the backstage affair
    Raymond summoned Suzanna during the lecture
    Penny blinded Carlos with the flashlight
    Danita treated Vivian for smallpox
    Meagan dunked Thomas under the water
    Theodore embarrassed Kimberly on Tuesday
    Russell outperformed Jasmine during the audition
    Jeremy nursed Brandon during the epidemic
    Peter disciplined Fran on the porch
    Kelsey mocked Alyssa during the vacation
    Drew frightened Paige during the ghost story
    Helen scared Jack with a Halloween mask
    Gus addressed Joy during the break
    Wayne passed Bree in the foyer
    Timothy pinched Allison during the ballet
    Lily clobbered Paul with a frying pan
    Dennis neglected Arthur during the inauguration
    Duke agitated Eve after practice
    Anne delivered Brett to the banquet
    Louis supported Rose throughout medical training
    Leo fastened Jay into the high chair
    Miriam saluted Olivia with a flag
    Kate surprised James during the Easter brunch
    Finn wanted April for the new position
    Abby humiliated Scott with the personal criticism
    Nick led Josh in the right direction
    Candace poked Griffin with the stick
    Dylan attacked Leah with a knife
    Ruth smacked Gary during the social
    Patrick corrupted Regina during summer camp
    Martha deceived Walter during the card game
    Scarlett transported Stephanie in the helicopter
    Jane nurtured Rita for a long time
    Todd tapped Nancy with the pencil
    Summer videotaped Farrah during the opera
    Howard teased Claudia at the anniversary gathering
    Ellen raised Martin onto the counter
    Patty told Dean about the stray cat
    Eddie badgered Shea about the mess
    Monica offended Victor at the family meal
    William married Madison in the fall
    Justin guarded Chloe in the bomb shelter
    Barbara divorced Lawrence after the accident
    Albert enlisted Glenn into the army
    Wendy thanked Sydney after the recital
    Brandy presented Sophie at the debutante ball
    Liz joined Eva before the debate
    Lisa ambushed Lana after the Thanksgiving feast
    Holly comforted Eliza during the war
    Joan avoided Susan for the entire semester
    Jill confronted Chaz in August
    Hilary entertained Lester during the harvest season
    Mary adopted Neve in December
    Noah examined Cindy on every other Saturday
    Ava slapped Rob during the rehearsal
    Dawn met Stacy three days ago
    Brittany annoyed Catherine throughout the holiday
    Kelly hushed Trudy before the training seminar
    Julie warned Andrea after prom
    Jessica spotted Valerie in the laboratory
    Herbert seduced Sandra in the pantry
    Tara arrested Marie in the parking garage
    Danny complimented Laura in the morning
    Kyle kicked Anita at the music festival
    Ian carried Ben down the steep mountainside
    Drake saw Brandt in the liquor store
    Derek insulted Brad in the shopping mall
    Phil questioned Pam during the oral exam
    Christy high-fived Hannah after the fight
    Faye followed Stan from a safe distance
    Troy discovered Dane at the shop
    Fred shoved Ernie on the playground
    Lauren called Robert from somewhere in Africa
    Grant bombarded Edgar with many questions
    Ethan took Brian to the theme park
    Bryson abandoned George in July
    Ken transferred Tom to another division
    Abe left Ron after nightfall
    Jake photographed Doug during the wedding reception
    Liam endorsed Jeff after the big expo
    Frank assisted Steve before bedtime
    Robbie paid Craig in September
    Dale hired Greg during the winter
    Kirk caught Matt behind the curtains
    Elise visited Annie in the recovery area
    Emily spanked Isaac in the small condo
    Edie recruited Sally for the basketball team
    Suzanne rejected Kristin after the big argument
    Bruce pushed Sophia into the closet
    Alisha fed Amanda with a small teaspoon
    Whitney noticed Krissy at the holiday gala
    Joe brought Amy to the job fair
    Theresa invited Joanna to the bridal shower
    Charlotte positioned Veronica next to the statue
    Lucy reprimanded Mindy in the alleyway
    Victoria lectured Danielle in the hair salon
    Christine recognized Evangeline at the reunion
    Margaret flattered Bethany during the dinner celebration
    Mason protected Karen in the car
    Earl contacted Ike last spring
    Alexis encouraged Betsy over the years
    Robin identified Becca at the hospital
    Phoebe escorted Katie around the compound
    Jacquelyn greeted Jennifer outside the movie theatre
    Shawn helped Lloyd into the back seat
    Michelle forgave Gustavo after a few weeks
    Charles punched Douglas while at college
    Desmond criticized Harold during the hearing
    Ryan scolded Hank after the birthday party
    Brenda praised Richard during the long chat
    Stanley watched Crystal inside the church
    Jacob inspired Trish during the performance
    Nicole applauded Calvin after the musical
    Mark adored Jenn during high school
    Kenneth stopped Matthew in the waiting room
    Eric defended Pablo from the attacker
    Jed observed Bob at the sales event
    Nelson congratulated Jerome after the project meeting
    Joseph approached Michael in the den
    Ted remembered Dirk during math class
    Ronald amused Neal in the crowded bar
    Will pointed Aiden toward the post office
    Erin interrogated Zack at the police headquarters
    Anthony scrutinized Heather during the workshop
    Table A3. Items From Experiments 3 and 4
    Active Passive
    Austin harassed Shelby at the market Shelby was harassed by Austin at the market
    Jason described Connor in the article Connor was described by Jason in the article
    Duke agitated Eve after practice Eve was agitated by Duke after practice
    Edie recruited Sally at the basketball finals Sally was recruited by Edie at the basketball finals
    Barbara pitied Stephanie after the accident Stephanie was pitied by Barbara after the accident
    Dylan attacked Leah in the basement Leah was attacked by Dylan in the basement
    Abby humiliated Scott in the break room Scott was humiliated by Abby in the break room
    Charlotte groomed Veronica at the nail salon Veronica was groomed by Charlotte at the nail salon
    Patrick corrupted Regina during summer camp Regina was corrupted by Patrick during summer camp
    Ethan interviewed Brian at the theme park Brian was interviewed by Ethan at the theme park
    Kate surprised James during the brunch James was surprised by Kate during the brunch
    Monica offended Victor at the family meal Victor was offended by Monica at the family meal
    Selena ogled Maggie at the backstage affair Maggie was ogled by Selena at the backstage affair
    Celeste astonished Owen during the function Owen was astonished by Celeste during the function
    Carl found Pete on the hiking trail Pete was found by Carl on the hiking trail
    Lyle chased Kayla into the pool Kayla was chased by Lyle into the pool
    Troy discovered Dane at the shop Dane was discovered by Troy at the shop
    Kelsey mocked Arthur during the vacation Arthur was mocked by Kelsey during the vacation
    Lily clobbered Paul in the kitchen Paul was clobbered by Lily in the kitchen
    Grace motivated Andrew during the workout Andrew was motivated by Grace during the workout
    Donald trailed Rachel through the city streets Rachel was trailed by Donald through the city streets
    Kenneth stopped Matthew in the waiting room Matthew was stopped by Kenneth in the waiting room
    Emilio spanked Isaac in the small condo Isaac was spanked by Emilio in the small condo
    Scarlett accompanied Lawrence to the safe house Lawrence was accompanied by Scarlett to the safe house
    Claire disturbed Tyrone during the big match Tyrone was disturbed by Claire during the big match
    Timothy pinched Allison during the ballet Allison was pinched by Timothy during the ballet
    Suzanne rejected Alisha after the big argument Alisha was rejected by Suzanne after the big argument
    Fred shoved Ernie on the playground Ernie was shoved by Fred on the playground
    Anne elbowed Brett at the banquet Brett was elbowed by Anne at the banquet
    Brandy presented Sophie at the debutante ball Sophie was presented by Brandy at the debutante ball
    Ian carried Ben after the fire Ben was carried by Ian after the fire
    Isabelle rescued Penelope from the burning building Penelope was rescued by Isabelle from the burning building
    Derek insulted Brad in the shopping mall Brad was insulted by Derek in the shopping mall
    Carly ignored Kevin during the funeral Kevin was ignored by Carly during the funeral
    Ryan scolded Hank after the play Hank was scolded by Ryan after the play
    Liam endorsed Jeff at the big expo Jeff was endorsed by Liam at the big expo
    Louis supported Rose throughout medical school Rose was supported by Louis throughout medical school
    Seth saw Ella on the television Ella was seen by Seth on the television
    Summer videotaped Farrah during the opera Farrah was videotaped by Summer during the opera
    Sharon banned Marcus from the lecture hall Marcus was banned by Sharon from the lecture hall
    Danita treated Vivian at the pharmacy Vivian was treated by Danita at the pharmacy
    Joseph approached Michael in the den Michael was approached by Joseph in the den
    Helen scared Jack at the campfire Jack was scared by Helen at the camp fire
    Albert beat Glenn during the weightlifting competition Glenn was beaten by Albert during the weightlifting competition
    Thelma bullied Chuck in the schoolyard Chuck was bullied by Thelma in the schoolyard
    Blaine mentioned Amber in the acceptance speech Amber was mentioned by Blaine in the acceptance speech
    Gus addressed Joy during the break Joy was addressed by Gus during the break
    Holly comforted Eliza during the war Eliza was comforted by Holly during the war
    Chad amazed Max during the fishing trip Max was amazed by Chad during the fishing trip
    Bryce struck Anna at the skating rink Anna was struck by Bryce at the skating rink
    Wayne nudged Bree in the foyer Bree was nudged by Wayne in the foyer
    Jared consulted Maria at the tax office Maria was consulted by Jared at the tax office
    Elise visited Annie in the recovery room Annie was visited by Elise in the recovery room
    Drake hailed Brandt in the liquor store Brandt was hailed by Drake in the liquor store
    Grant soothed Edgar at the zoo Edgar was soothed by Grant at the zoo
    Drew frightened Paige at the movie theater Paige was frightened by Drew at the movie theater
    Kristin fed Amanda at lunchtime Amanda was fed by Kristin at lunchtime
    Johnny woke Melissa before work Melissa was woken by Johnny before work
    Robin identified Katie at the hospital Katie was identified by Robin at the hospital
    Miriam saluted Olivia during the parade Olivia was saluted by Miriam during the parade
    Trent welcomed Chase into the new house Chase was welcomed by Trent into the new house
    Lynn electrocuted Marcia at the power plant Marcia was electrocuted by Lynn at the power plant
    Beck corrected Hugh during the pop quiz Hugh was corrected by Beck during the pop quiz
    Penny blinded Carlos on the highway Carlos was blinded by Penny on the highway
    Sarah bothered Luke at the dance club Luke was bothered by Sarah at the dance club
    Meagan soaked Thomas at the birthday party Thomas was soaked by Meagan at the birthday
    Kurt located Gale at the wedding ceremony Gale was located by Kurt at the wedding ceremony
    Ruth smacked Gary during the social Gary was smacked by Ruth during the social
    Mona startled Roger in the funhouse Roger was startled by Mona in the funhouse
    Ted pestered Dirk during math class Dirk was pestered by Ted during math class
    Candace poked Griffin in the emergency room Griffin was poked by Candace in the emergency room
    Brittany annoyed Catherine at the holiday gathering Catherine was annoyed by Brittany at the holiday gathering
    Finn fired April after the incident April was fired by Finn after the incident
    Peter berated Fran during basic training Fran was berated by Peter during basic training
    Eugene intimidated Violet before the soccer championship Violet was intimidated by Eugene before the soccer championship
    Wade accused Kat during the trial Kat was accused by Wade during the trial
    Victoria repulsed Danielle in the hair salon Danielle was repulsed by Victoria in the hair salon
    Jane nurtured Rita for a long time Rita was nurtured by Jane for a long time
    Nathan served Carmella at the diner Carmella was served by Nathan at the diner
    Elizabeth dismissed Alexandra from the meeting Alexandra was dismissed by Elizabeth from the meeting
    Todd sterilized Nancy before the operation Nancy was sterilized by Todd before the operation
    Jacquelyn greeted Evangeline outside the movie theatre Evangeline was greeted by Jacquelyn outside the movie theatre
    Abe left Ron at the penitentiary Ron was left by Abe at the penitentiary
    Liz joined Eva before the debate Eva was joined by Liz before the debate
    Jessica spotted Valerie in the laboratory Valerie was spotted by Jessica in the laboratory
    Dennis neglected Alyssa during the inauguration Alyssa was neglected by Dennis during the inauguration
    Eric defended Pablo during the prison riot Pablo was defended by Eric during the prison riot
    Jacob inspired Trish during the performance Trish was inspired by Jacob during the performance
    Erin interrogated Zack at the police headquarters Zack was interrogated by Erin at the police headquarters
    Michelle forgave Gustavo after a few weeks Gustavo was forgiven by Michelle after a few weeks
    Herbert seduced Sandra in the pantry Sandra was seduced by Herbert in the pantry
    Christy lauded Hannah after the fight Hannah was lauded by Christy after the fight
    Earl contacted Ike last spring Ike was contacted by Earl last spring
    Suzanna summoned Raymond during the lecture Raymond was summoned by Suzanna during the lecture
    Jill confronted Chaz in the laundry room Chaz was confronted by Jill in the laundry room
    Miranda hugged Dominic at the concert Dominic was hugged by Miranda at the concert
    Julie warned Andrea after the prom Andrea was warned by Julie after the prom
    Faith taunted Bill during recess Bill was taunted by Faith during recess
    Will numbed Aiden at the dental clinic Aiden was numbed by Will at the dental clinic
    Jed observed Bob at the sales event Bob was observed by Jed at the sales event
    Noah examined Cindy at the free clinic Cindy was examined by Noah at the free clinic
    William pacified Madison after the crisis Madison was pacified by William after the crisis
    Sean helped Lloyd at the fundraiser Lloyd was helped by Sean at the fundraiser
    Leo embraced Jay at the award ceremony Jay was embraced by Leo at the award ceremony
    Anthony scrutinized Heather during the workshop Heather was scrutinized by Anthony during the workshop
    Nora pursued David during the fall term David was pursued by Nora during the fall term
    Kelly hushed Trudy before the training seminar Trudy was hushed by Kelly before the training seminar
    Desmond criticized Charles during the hearing Charles was criticized by Desmond during the hearing
    Danny complimented Laura in the morning Laura was complimented by Danny in the morning
    Rebecca acknowledged Ashley at the football game Ashley was acknowledged by Rebecca at the football game
    Whitney noticed Krissy at the holiday gala Krissy was noticed by Whitney at the holiday gala
    Stanley watched Crystal inside the church Crystal was watched by Stanley inside the church
    Ellen kissed Martin on the cheek Martin was kissed by Ellen on the cheek
    Joan avoided Susan for the entire semester Susan was avoided by Joan for the entire semester
    Margaret flattered Bethany during the dinner celebration Bethany was flattered by Margaret during the dinner celebration
    Kyle kicked Pam at the music festival Pam was kicked by Kyle at the music festival
    Bruce pushed Sophia into the closet Spohia was pushed by Bruce into the closet
    Phoebe escorted Becca around the compound Becca was escorted by Phoebe around the compound
    Phil questioned Anita during the oral exam Anita was questioned by Phil during the oral exam
    Howard teased Claudia at the annual gathering Claudia was teased by Howard at the annual gathering
    Mark adored Jenn during high school Jenn was adored by Mark during high school
    Tara arrested Marie in the parking garage Marie was arrested by Tara in the parking garage
    Faye spied Stan in the crowd Stan was spied by Faye in the crowd
    Bryson abandoned George in the parking lot George was abandoned by Bryson in the parking lot
    Nicole applauded Calvin after the musical Calvin was applauded by Nicole after the musical
    Martha deceived Wendy during the card game Wendy was deceived by Martha during the card game
    Mason protected Karen in the car Karen was protected by Mason in the car
    Dawn ambushed Lana after the feast Lana was ambushed by Dawn after the feast
    Brenda praised Richard during the long chat Richard was praised by Brenda during the long chat
    Hilary entertained Lester at the karaoke bar Lester was entertained by Hilary at the karaoke bar
    Eddie badgered Shea in the mess hall Shea was badgered by Eddie in the mess hall
    Joshua selected Amy at the job fair Amy was selected by Joshua at the job fair
    Ken shushed Tom at the board meeting Tom was shushed by Ken at the board meeting
    Dale hired Greg after the job search Greg was hired by Dale after the job search
    Nelson congratulated Jerome after the project meeting Jerome was congratulated by Nelson after the project meeting
    Beatrice hit Samantha at the stop sign Samantha was hit by Beatrice at the stop sign
    Robbie paid Craig at the checkout counter Craig was paid by Robbie at the checkout counter
    Nadine reassured Darryl after the carnival Darryl was reassured by Nadine after the carnival
    Patty emailed Dean during the lecture Dean was emailed by Patty during the lecture
    Alexis encouraged Betsy in the locker room Betsy was encouraged by Alexis in the locker room
    June cornered Cole under the tree Cole was cornered by June under the tree
    Frank assisted Steve before bedtime Steve was assisted by Frank before bedtime
    Jeremy nursed Brandon during the epidemic Brandon was nursed by Jeremy during the epidemic
    Justin guarded Chloe in the bomb shelter Chloe was guarded by Justin in the bomb shelter
    Ronald amused Neal in the crowded apartment Neal was amused by Ronald in the crowded apartment
    Jake photographed Doug during the wedding reception Doug was photographed by Jake during the wedding reception
    Jasmine outperformed Russell at the audition Russell was outperformed by Jasmine at the audition
    Carolina belittled Fredrick at the business assembly Fredrick was belittled by Carolina at the business assembly
    Mary uplifted Neve during the winter months Neve was uplifted by Mary during the winter months
    Nick led Josh to the amusement park Josh was led by Nick to the amusement park
    Walter thanked Sydney after the recital Sydney was thanked by Walter after the recital
    Lauren phoned Robert from somewhere in Africa Robert was phoned by Lauren from somewhere in Africa
    Kirk caught Matt behind the curtains Matt was caught by Kirk behind the curtains
    Christine recognized Jennifer at the reunion Jennifer was recognized by Christine at the reunion
    Theodore embarrassed Kimberly during the camping trip Kimberly was embarrassed by Theodore during the camping trip
    Theresa pranked Joanna at the bridal shower Joanna was pranked by Theresa at the bridal shower
    Lisa tickled Stacy at daycare Stacy was tickled by Lisa at daycare
    Lucy reprimanded Mindy in the alleyway Mindy was reprimanded by Lucy in the alleyway
    Harold punched Douglas at the bar Douglas was punched by Harold at the bar
    Ava slapped Rob during the rehearsal Rob was slapped by Ava during the rehearsal

      The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.