MORPHINE TOLERANCE AS A FUNCTION OF RATIO SCHEDULE: RESPONSE REQUIREMENT OR UNIT PRICE?
Corresponding Author
Christine E. Hughes
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, 601 S. College Road, Wilmington, North Carolina 28403-5612 (e-mail: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorStacey C. Sigmon
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorRaymond C. Pitts
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorLinda A. Dykstra
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Christine E. Hughes
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, 601 S. College Road, Wilmington, North Carolina 28403-5612 (e-mail: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorStacey C. Sigmon
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorRaymond C. Pitts
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorLinda A. Dykstra
UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Key pecking by 3 pigeons was maintained by a multiple fixed-ratio 10, fixed-ratio 30, fixed-ratio 90 schedule of food presentation. Components differed with respect to amount of reinforcement, such that the unit price was 10 responses per 1-s access to food. Acute administration of morphine, l-methadone, and cocaine dose-dependently decreased overall response rates in each of the components. When a rate decreasing dose of morphine was administered daily, tolerance, as measured by an increase in the dose that reduced response rates to 50% of control (i.e., the ED50 value), developed in each of the components; however, the degree of tolerance was smallest in the fixed-ratio 90 component (i.e., the ED50 value increased the least). When the l-methadone dose-effect curve was redetermined during the chronic morphine phase, the degree of cross-tolerance conferred to l-methadone was similar across components, suggesting that behavioral variables may not influence the degree of cross-tolerance between opioids. During the chronic phase, the cocaine dose-effect curve shifted to the right for 2 pigeons and to the left for 1 pigeon, which is consistent with predictions based on the lack of pharmacological similarity between morphine and cocaine. When the morphine, l-methadone, and cocaine dose-effect curves were redetermined after chronic morphine administration ended, the morphine and l-methadone ED50s replicated those obtained prior to chronic morphine administration. The morphine data suggest that the fixed-ratio value (i.e., the absolute output) determines the degree of tolerance and not the unit price.
REFERENCES
- Bickel, W. K., DeGrandpre, R. J., Higgins, S. T., & Hughes, J. R. (1990). Behavioral economics of drug self-administration. I. Functional equivalence of response requirement and drug dose. Life Sciences, 47, 1501–1510.
- Bickel, W. K., DeGrandpre, R. J., Hughes, J. R., & Higgins, S. T. (1991). Behavioral economics of drug self-administration. II. A unit-price analysis of cigarette smoking. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 145–154.
- Branch, M. N. (1990). Cocaine tolerance: Interactions among random-ratio and random-interval reinforcement-schedule parameters and repeated exposure to cocaine. Drug Development Research, 20, 19–30.
- Brocco, M. J., & McMillan, D. E. (1983). Tolerance to d-amphetamine and lack of cross-tolerance to other drugs in rats under a multiple schedule of food presentation. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 24, 34–39.
- Collier, G. H., Johnson, D. F., Hill, W. L., & Kaufman, L. W. (1986). The economics of the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 113–136.
- Craft, R. M., Picker, M. J., & Dykstra, L. A. (1989). Differential cross-tolerance to opioid agonists in morphine-tolerant pigeons responding under a schedule of food presentation. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 249, 386–393.
- DeGrandpre, R. J., Bickel, W. K., Hughes, J. R., Layng, M. P., & Badger, G. (1993). Unit price as a useful metric in analyzing effects of reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 641–666.
- English, J. A., Rowlett, J. K., & Woolverton, W. L. (1995). Unit-price analysis of opioid consumption under a progressive-ratio schedule of drug injection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 361–371.
- Epstein, R. (1981). Amount consumed as a function of magazine-cycle duration. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 41, 63–66.
- Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
- Foltin, R., & Schuster, C. R. (1982). Behavioral tolerance and cross-tolerance to dl-cathinone and d-amphetamine in rats. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 22, 126–131.
- Foster, T. A., & Hackenberg, T. D. (2004). Unit price and choice in a token-reinforcement context. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 5–25.
-
A. J. Goudie, &
M. W. Emmett-Oglesby (Eds.). (1989). Psychoactive drugs: Tolerance and sensitization. Clifton, NJ: The Humana Press.
10.1385/0896031489 Google Scholar
- Heifetz, S. A., & McMillan, D. E. (1971). Development of behavioral tolerance to morphine and methadone using the schedule-controlled behavior of the pigeon. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.), 19, 40–52.
- Hoffman, S. H., Branch, M. N., & Sizemore, G. M. (1987). Cocaine tolerance: Acute versus chronic effects as dependent upon fixed-ratio size. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 363–376.
- Hughes, C. E., & Branch, M. N. (1991). Tolerance to and residual effects of cocaine in squirrel monkeys depend on reinforcement-schedule parameter. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 345–360.
- Hughes, C. E., Dykstra, L. A., & Picker, M. J. (1996). Behavioral tolerance and cross-tolerance to the response rate-decreasing effects of mu opioids in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 7, 228–236.
- Hughes, C. E., Picker, M. J., & Dykstra, L. A. (1995). Tolerance and cross-tolerance to the response rate-decreasing effects of mu opioids in morphine-maintained squirrel monkeys. Behavioural Pharmacology, 6, 776–784.
- Hughes, C. E., Pitts, R. C., & Branch, M. N. (1996). Cocaine and food deprivation: Effects on food-reinforced fixed-ratio performance in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 145–158.
- Hursh, S. R. (1980). Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219–238.
- Hursh, S. R. (1984). Behavioral economics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 435–452.
- Hursh, S. R., Raslear, T. G., Shurtleff, D., Bauman, R., & Simmon, L. (1988). A cost benefit analysis of demand for food. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 419–440.
- Hursh, S. R., & Winger, G. (1995). Normalized demand for drugs and other reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 373–384.
- Jarema, K., Macomber, C., LeSage, M. G., & Poling, A. (1999). Acute and chronic effects of morphine under a progressive-ratio 25 schedule of food delivery. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 62, 209–214.
- Madden, G. J., Bickel, W. K., & Jacobs, E. A. (2000). Three predictions of the economic concept of unit price in a choice context. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 45–64.
- Nickel, M., & Poling, A. (1990). Fixed-ratio size as a determinant of the development of tolerance to morphine. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1, 463–467.
- Oliveto, A. H., Picker, M. J., & Dykstra, L. A. (1991). Acute and chronic morphine administration: Effects of mixed-action opioids in rats and squirrel monkeys responding under a schedule of food presentation. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 257, 8–18.
- Picker, M. J., Negus, S. S., & Powell, K. R. (1991). Differential cross-tolerance to mu and kappa opioid agonists in morphine-tolerant rats responding under a schedule of food presentation. Psychopharmacology, 103, 129–135.
- Poling, A., Byrne, T., Christian, L., & LeSage, M. G. (2000). Effects of cocaine and morphine under mixed-ratio schedules of food delivery: Support for a behavioral momentum analysis. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 66, 313–321.
- Poling, A., LeSage, M. G., Roe, D., & Schaefer, D. (1996). Acute and chronic effects of morphine in pigeons responding under a progressive-ratio schedule of food delivery. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 54, 485–490.
- Sannerud, C. A., Marley, R. J., Serdikoff, S. L., Alastra, A. J. G., Cohen, C., & Goldberg, S. R. (1993). Tolerance to the behavioral effects of chlordiazepoxide: Pharmacological and biochemical selectivity. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 267, 1311–1320.
- Sannerud, C. A., & Young, A. M. (1986). Modification of morphine tolerance by behavioral variables. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 237, 75–81.
- Schama, K. F., & Branch, M. N. (1989). Tolerance to effects of cocaine on schedule-controlled behavior: Effects of fixed-interval schedule parameter. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 32, 267–274.
- Smith, J. B. (1978). Effects of d-amphetamine and pentobarbital in combination with single and repeated daily injections of morphine in the pigeon. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 206, 353–360.
- Smith, J. B. (1986a). Effects of chronically administered d-amphetamine on spaced responding maintained under multiple and single-component schedules. Psychopharmacology, 88, 296–300.
- Smith, J. B. (1986b). Effects of fixed-ratio length on the development of tolerance to decreased responding by l-nantradol. Psychopharmacology, 90, 259–262.
- Smith, J. B. (1990). Effects of fixed-ratio requirement on observed tolerance to decreased responding by clonidine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 34, 993–995.
- Smith, J. B. (1991). Effects of shock intensity on observed tolerance to decreased avoidance responding by clonidine. Psychopharmacology, 103, 268–270.
- Stallings, M. E., & Hughes, C. E. (1999, May). Effects of repeated administration of morphine on response rates as a function of unit price in pigeons. Poster presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Chicago, IL.
- Woolverton, W. L., Kandel, D., & Schuster, C. R. (1978). Tolerance and cross-tolerance to cocaine and d-amphetamine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 205, 525–535.
- Yoon, J. H., & Branch, M. N. (2004). Interactions among unit price, fixed-ratio value, and dosing regimen in determining effects of repeated cocaine administration. Behavioural Processes, 67, 363–381.
- Young, A. M., & Griffin, A. C. (1990). Magnitude of initial effect influences development of tolerance to morphine in rats responding under a fixed-ratio schedule of food presentation. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1, 531–539.
- Young, A. M., Kapitsopoulos, G., & Makhay, M. M. (1991). Tolerance to morphine-like stimulus effects of mu opioid agonists. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 257, 795–805.