Relationship between hearing, cognitive function, and quality of life in aging companion dogs
Margaret E. Gruen and Natasha J. Olby are joint senior authors.
Abstract
Background
Elderly people with presbycusis are at higher risk for dementia and depression than the general population. There is no information regarding consequences of presbycusis in dogs.
Objective
Evaluate the relationship between cognitive function, quality of life, and hearing loss in aging companion dogs.
Animals
Thirty-nine elderly companion dogs.
Methods
Prospective study. Hearing was evaluated using brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER) testing. Dogs were grouped by hearing ability. Owners completed the canine dementia scale (CADES) and canine owner-reported quality of life (CORQ) questionnaire. Cognitive testing was performed, and cognitive testing outcomes, CADES and CORQ scores and age were compared between hearing groups.
Results
Nineteen dogs could hear at 50 dB, 12 at 70 dB, and 8 at 90 dB with mean ages (months) of 141 ± 14, 160 ± 16, and 172 ± 15 for each group respectively (P = .0002). Vitality and companionship CORQ scores were significantly lower as hearing deteriorated (6.6-5.4, 50-90 dB group, P = .03 and 6.9-6.2, 50-90 dB group, P = .02, respectively). Cognitive classification by CADES was abnormal in all 90 dB group dogs and normal in 3/12 70 dB group and 11/19 50 dB group dogs (P = .0004). Performance on inhibitory control, detour and sustained gaze tasks decreased significantly with hearing loss (P = .001, P = .008, P = .002, respectively). In multivariate analysis, higher CADES score was associated with worse hearing (P = .01).
Conclusions and Clinical Importance
Presbycusis negatively alters owner-pet interactions and is associated with poor executive performance and owner-assessed dementia severity.
Abbreviations
-
- BAER
-
- brainstem auditory evoked response
-
- CADES
-
- Canine Dementia Scale
-
- CCDS
-
- canine cognitive dysfunction syndrome
-
- CMI
-
- clinical metrology instruments
-
- CORQ
-
- canine owner reported quality of life
-
- CVM
-
- College of Veterinary Medicine
-
- dB
-
- decibel
-
- MRI
-
- magnetic resonance imaging
-
- QoL
-
- quality of life
1 INTRODUCTION
Age-related hearing loss, or presbycusis, is a common condition in the elderly human population with serious consequences. It is estimated that one-third of people over the age 65 experience presbycusis.1 The rate of cognitive decline is approximately 30% to 40% faster in people with presbycusis2 and the risk for dementia was highest for people with age-related hearing loss when compared to other risk factors including hypertension, obesity, and poorer education.3 The sensory deprivation caused by hearing loss is associated with social isolation and depression, which can lead to a decreased quality of life (QoL) for many elderly individuals.4-6 However, the use of hearing aids and cochlear implants can counteract progression of presbycusis and help mitigate many of the changes seen with cognitive decline and depression in the elderly population.7-10
Presbycusis is also a common phenomenon in the aging process of dogs starting at approximately 8 to 10 years of age, with significant hearing loss occurring in middle to high frequencies when compared to low frequencies.11 Changes in audiograms can be assessed using brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER) testing.12 Certain histopathological changes in the human cochlea have been associated with loss of hearing on audiogram profiles. These changes typically are divided into 6 different categories: sensory, neural, metabolic, cochlear conductive, mixed, and indeterminate.13 Most cases of presbycusis in humans are categorized as mixed although approximately 25% of cases are classified as indeterminate.13-15 Similarly, sensory, neural, metabolic, and mixed changes have been reported in dogs with presbycusis.12, 16, 17
The consequences of presbycusis in companion animals have not yet been thoroughly studied. A previous study found that owner-assessed sensory impairment, including hearing loss, resulted in a higher number of problematic behaviors that may reflect canine cognitive dysfunction syndrome (CCDS),18 but limited information is available regarding the effect of presbycusis on QoL and cognitive function via testing. Various validated methods have been developed to evaluate both QoL and cognitive function in dogs including clinical metrology instruments (CMI) completed by owners and cognitive testing.19-24 Quality of life is 1 of the most important factors for pet owners when considering end of life decisions.25, 26 Previous research has focused on QoL assessment related to treatments or diseases, but no data have been published regarding owner perception of the effect of presbycusis on their pet's QoL.27-29 Evaluation of cognitive function and QoL in dogs with presbycusis warrants further investigation to improve our understanding and treatment of aging dogs, and to explore the relationship between sensory loss and cognition.
The purpose of our study was to evaluate the relationships among aging, cognitive function, QoL, and hearing loss in old companion dogs. We hypothesized that hearing, as evaluated by BAER testing, would be positively associated with cognitive performance and with the owner's assessment of QoL in aging companion dogs.
2 METHODS
2.1 Study population
Ours was a cross-sectional study of prospectively recruited companion dogs at the North Carolina (NC) State University College of Veterinary Medicine (CVM). Dogs were recruited by contacting owners in the local community and the NC State CVM through emails and postings on the NC State CVM clinical trials website. Dogs were recruited from January 2019 through May 2021. Dogs had to be in the last 25% or beyond their expected lifespan according to American Kennel Club breed standards to participate.30 Dogs in the last 25% of their expected lifespan were considered senior dogs and dogs beyond their expected lifespan were considered geriatric. American Kennel Club lifespans were determined for mixed breed dogs by matching them to the breed that they most closely resembled within their weight class. To be included in the study, dogs had to be systemically healthy, temperamentally suited to cognitive testing, and food motivated for completion of cognitive and hearing testing. Exclusion criteria included congenital or acquired deafness for genetic or other reasons before reaching the senior age category, recent initiation (defined as <4 weeks) of psychoactive medications that might alter behavioral testing, and severe mobility or visual impairment that would prevent cognitive testing. All protocols were reviewed and approved by the NC State University Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee and all owners reviewed and signed an informed consent form.
2.2 Clinical examination
All dogs underwent general physical, orthopedic, and neurological examinations by a veterinarian, and a CBC, serum biochemistry panel, and urine analysis were performed.
2.3 Auditory testing
Hearing was assessed in a quiet room using brainstem auditory evoked potentials (Nicolet VikingQuest, Middleton, Wisconsin). Before recording, the external ear canal was examined, and excessive debris was removed manually. Dogs unable to sit still throughout the BAER examination received sedation appropriate for the dog's demeanor and underlying health status. Dogs received either 1 to 7 mg/kg trazodone PO (Teva Laboratories, Pliva, Zagreb, Croatia), 0.01 mg/kg acepromazine IV (Covetrus, Dublin, Ohio), 0.2 mg/kg butorphanol IV (Hospira, Lake Forest, Illinois), 3 μg/kg dexmedetomidine IV (Zoetis, Kalamazoo, Michigan), 20 mg/kg gabapentin PO (Zydus Pharmaceuticals, Pennington, New Jersey), or a combination of these medications before BAER evaluation. The presence of wave V was used to determine whether a dog could hear at a particular decibel (dB) level. Wave V was defined as the wave immediately preceding the deepest trough that occurred approximately 3.5 to 5 ms after stimulation according to normal convention.31 Wave V is the last waveform to disappear with decreasing stimulus levels and therefore was chosen as the marker of hearing in our study.32 Reference electrodes were inserted SC over the mastoid process at the base of the vertical ear canal bilaterally. The recording electrode was inserted SC at the vertex. Finally, a ground electrode was inserted SC dorsal to the first cervical vertebra. The electrodes were connected to a preamplifier with 0.15 to 3 kHz bandpass filtering. Tubal inserts with earplugs were seated in both external ear canals to deliver the stimuli. Click stimuli were delivered monaurally at a stimulation rate of 11.4 Hz using an alternating polarity starting at 70 dB. The response to 1000 clicks was recorded and averaged for each recording. A masking noise at 40 dB was used in the non-stimulated ear in all examinations. If a clear wave V was identified in testing of either ear, the intensity of stimulus was decreased to 50 dB and testing was repeated. If no waveforms were identified at 70 dB, the intensity of stimulus was increased to 90 dB and testing was repeated (Figure 1). Testing was started at 70 dB rather than 50 dB to minimize the number of trials required for testing (3 rather than 2). The veterinarians assessing dogs' hearing were not blinded to the results of the questionnaire scores or performance on cognitive tasks.

2.4 Questionnaires
Owners were asked whether they believed their dogs had any hearing problems before BAER evaluation. Owners also were asked to provide any relevant medical history and to include current medications that their dogs were receiving. None of the dogs were noted to have otitis externa that required topical aural treatment at the time of enrollment. Additionally, owners filled out an adapted canine owner-reported quality of life (CORQ) questionnaire. This scale was validated to measure QoL in dogs being treated for cancer.24 The questionnaire includes 17 questions assigned to 4 domains which include vitality, companionship, pain, and mobility. One change made to the questionnaire was related to question 5: “My dog's treatment interfered with his/her enjoyment of life.” Instead, the word “treatment” was replaced with “anxiety.” All other questions were related to general changes seen with aging and not specific to cancer treatment or course of disease (Data S1). Each item was scored from 0 to 7 based on the number of days in the past week that the owner witnessed a specific behavior such as decreased appetite, difficulty lying down, or sleeping well at night. The items were scored such that higher scores were associated with a more positive response. The scores for each domain were calculated by averaging the scores of all questions pertaining to the domain. The overall score was the average of all item responses, and all scores ranged from 0 to 7.
To assess cognitive dysfunction, owners completed the canine dementia scale (CADES) questionnaire within 2 weeks of evaluation (Data S2). The CADES questionnaire is a validated CMI used to assess the severity of CCDS based on the owners' perceptions of behavior at home.19 Owners are asked to answer 17 items related to behavioral changes that evaluate 4 domains—spatial orientation, social interaction, sleep-wake cycles, and house soiling. Dogs then were assigned different clinical stages of CCDS based on their total score: normal cognitive function (0-7), mild (8-23), moderate (24-44), and severe (45-95) cognitive impairment.
2.5 Cognitive testing
Clinical cognitive testing was performed at the NC State CVM comparative behavior laboratory. Cognitive testing was performed within 4 weeks of BAER testing. Testing was performed in the same testing room with the same dog handler and was recorded by 2 digital video cameras for future analyses. Cognitive tests including warm-ups, social cue (pointing), working memory, cylinder tasks (inhibitory control and detour), and sustained gaze methodology are described in Supporting Information (Data S3). All test methods have been described previously.22, 33-36
2.6 Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using JMP15 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina). Dogs were grouped based on the lowest dB reading with an identifiable wave V (50, 70, or 90 dB) and summary data were generated for each group for age, breed, weight, sex, CORQ, CADES, and cognitive testing. Data for pointing cue and cylinder tasks were expressed as a percentage of correct choices. Data for sustained attention were expressed as the mean timespan (s) of 3 trials. Canine dementia scale was expressed both as a numerical score and as a category, and working memory was expressed as a category. Continuous data are presented as median and range (if not normally distributed) and mean and SD (if normally distributed). Categorical data are expressed as numbers and percentages.
Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to compare age, outcomes from cognitive testing, and CORQ scores between hearing groups. Chi square analyses were used to compare sex, working memory categories, and CADES categories between hearing groups. The relative risk of dementia in dogs with and without hearing loss was calculated by dividing the proportion of dogs identified as having moderate or severe CCDS on CADES in the 90 dB group by the proportion of dogs in the same CADES categories in the 50 and 70 dB groups. All outcomes that were significant using the Kruskal-Wallis tests were assessed by pairwise comparisons. Effect sizes were determined using Cohen's d for pairwise comparisons.
The influence of age on relevant cognitive testing, CADES CCDS scores and CORQ scores was examined using simple linear regression. Given the influence of age on cognitive performance and CORQ scores, linear regression models were constructed to incorporate age into multivariate analysis of cognitive testing results, CADES CCDS scores, CORQ scores, and hearing levels. Effect sizes for significant correlations were determined using Cohen's f2. P values ≤.05 were considered significant.
3 RESULTS
3.1 Dogs
Thirty-nine dogs (25 spayed females and 14 castrated males) participated in the study. Breeds represented include Labrador retriever (n = 5), beagle (n = 3), border collie (n = 2), golden retriever (n = 2), dachshund (n = 2), American Staffordshire terrier (n = 2), German shepherd dog (n = 1), Jack Russell Terrier (n = 1), Brittany spaniel (n = 1), Siberian husky (n = 1), Bernese mountain dog (n = 1), West Highland white terrier (n = 1), German short-haired pointer (n = 1), hound (n = 1), and mixed breed (n = 16). The median age of dogs in the study was 156 months (range, 115-197 months).
All dogs had essentially normal physical examinations, although age-related changes such as osteoarthritis were common. All dogs were independently mobile, visual, and able to respond to commands. One dog had residual signs of peripheral vestibular disease, 1 dog had a vestibular quality ataxia, and 1 dog had new onset of seizures during enrollment. Brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) did not identify any structural disease in the dog with ataxia or the dog with seizures. No other dogs had any clinical signs of focal intracranial neurological disease. Otoscopic examination of the external ear canal did not identify any obvious cause for conductive hearing loss in any of the dogs.
Blood testing was performed within 1 week of cognitive testing in 30 dogs and the remaining dogs had blood tests performed within 6 months before testing. The most common findings included mild non-regenerative anemia, evidence of mild to moderate mixed hepatopathies, and urinary tract infections (all dogs had cultures performed and were treated with antibiotics as necessary before participation). A summary of the results of CBC, biochemistry, and urinalyses for all dogs are provided in Supporting Information (Data S4-S6, respectively).
3.2 Auditory testing
Brainstem auditory evoked response testing was completed successfully in all dogs. To complete the BAER test, 15 dogs did not require any form of sedation, whereas the remaining 24 dogs were given some sedation with the drug choice depending on their health status and level of anxiety. Eighteen dogs received trazodone, 2 dogs received dexmedetomidine, 1 dog received gabapentin, 2 dogs received trazodone and dexmedetomidine, and 1 dog received trazodone, acepromazine, and butorphanol. Nineteen dogs had wave V at 50 dB, 12 at 70 dB, and 5 at 90 dB. Two dogs that had no waveforms at 70 dB but were not tested at 90 dB were placed in the 90 dB group, as well as 1 dog that had no waveforms at 90 dB in either ear, to form a group with decreased to absent hearing for statistical analysis. For the dogs in the 50 dB group, 17% of owners thought their dogs had hearing problems, compared with 75% of owners in both the 70 and 90 dB groups. Summary statistics for age, weight, and sex in each hearing group are provided in Table 1. A significant difference in age (P = .0002) was found between hearing groups but not for sex (P = .35) or weight (P = .5). On pairwise comparison between the groups, dogs were significantly older in the 70 and 90 dB groups compared to the 50 dB group (P = .004, d = .54 and P = .0003, d = .73, respectively) but no significant difference in age was found between the 70 and 90 dB groups.
50 dB group | 70 dB group | 90 dB group | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Number of dogs | 19 | 12 | 8 | |
Sex | m = 9, f = 10 | m = 3, f = 8 | m = 2, f = 7 | .35 |
Age (mean, SD) (m) | 141.1 ± 14.04 | 160.3 ± 15.56 | 172 ± 15.04 | .0002 |
Weight (mean, SD) (kg) | 21.8 ± 9.54 | 20.89 ± 9.25 | 17.3 ± 7.2 | .5 |
- Abbreviations: dB, decibel; f, females; m, males; m, months.
3.3 Quality of life assessment
The CORQ was completed by 35/39 (89.7%) of owners. Three of the dogs without a CORQ were in the 90 dB group and the remaining dog was in the 50 dB group. No significant difference in total CORQ scores was found between hearing groups (Table 2). Of the 4 CORQ domains, a significant difference was found between hearing groups in the domains of vitality (P = .03) and companionship (P = .02) with both vitality and companionship scores decreasing as hearing thresholds increased (hearing loss; Table 2). Pairwise comparisons between groups identified significantly higher CORQ vitality scores for dogs in the 70 and 90 dB groups compared to the 50 dB groups (P = .02, d = .43 and P = .004, d = .59, respectively; Figure 2A). Similarly, a significantly higher CORQ companionship score was found for dogs in the 50 dB group compared to the 90 dB group (P = .004, d = 1.44; Figure 2B). No differences in scores for pain (P = .05) and mobility (P = .17) domains were found between hearing groups (Table 2).
50 dB group | 70 dB group | 90 dB group | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
CORQ total (median) (range) | 6.62 (4.88-7) | 5.91 (3.47-6.82) | 6 (3.12-6.82) | .06 |
CORQ vitality (median) (range) | 6.6 (4.6-7) | 5.6 (2.6-7) | 5.4 (2.6-6.8) | .03 |
CORQ comp (median) (range) | 6.9 (6.3-7) | 6.75 (3.5-7) | 6.2 (4.5-6.7) | .02 |
CORQ pain (median) (range) | 6.5 (4-7) | 5.25 (3.5-7) | 5.5 (3.5-7) | .05 |
CORQ mobility (median) (range) | 6.63 (3-7) | 5.5 (1.5-7) | 5.5 (0.5-7) | .17 |
- Note: Higher CORQ scores indicate better perceived quality of life.
- Abbreviations: comp, companionship; CORQ, Canine Owner-Reported Quality of Life; dB, decibel.

3.4 Cognitive dysfunction assessment by CADES
Owner-completed CADES scoring classified 4 dogs as having severe CCDS, 10 dogs as having moderate CCDS, 11 dogs as having mild CCDS, and 13 dogs as normal. One owner of a dog in the 50 dB group did not fill out the CADES questionnaire. All dogs identified as severe CCDS fell into the 90 dB group. Of the normal dogs, 11/13 (84.6%) could hear at 50 dB whereas the other 2 could hear at 70 dB (Figure 3). A significant difference in CADES scores was found across hearing groups (P = .0004; Table 3). The relative risk for dogs in the 90 dB group to be identified as moderate or severe CCDS on CADES was 3.88.

50 dB group | 70 dB group | 90 dB group | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
CADES score (range) | 6 (0-40) | 26.5 (3-44) | 41 (21-70) | .0004 |
Pointing cue (avg % of correct choices) (range) | 83 (17-100) | 87.5 (58-100) | 92 (83-100) | .74 |
Working memory (Grade 1, <20 s), (Grade 2, 20-60 s), (Grade 3, >60 s) | Grade 1, n = 5 | Grade 1, n = 2 | Grade 1, n = 2 | .80 |
Grade 2, n = 7 | Grade 2, n = 5 | Grade 2, n = 3 | ||
Grade 3, n = 4 | Grade 3, n = 1 | Grade 1, n = 1 | ||
Inhibitory control (avg % of correct trials) (range) | 100 (63-100) | 88 (13-100) | 50 (0-88) | .001 |
Detour (avg % of correct trials) (range) | 75 (0-100) | 38 (0-75) | 13 (0-50) | .008 |
Sustained gaze (avg from three trials) (range) (s) | 21.72 (5.83-60) | 16.23 (2.99-53.8) | 7.7 (0-20.75) | .02 |
- Abbreviations: CADES, Canine Dementia Scale; dB, decibel; s, seconds.
3.5 Cognitive test results
Thirty-one of 39 dogs passed the warm-up criteria for pointing and working memory tasks. Of the 8 dogs that failed the warm-up tasks, 4 were in the 90 dB group, 2 were in the 70 dB group, and the remaining 2 dogs were in the 50 dB group. Six of the dogs that did not pass the warm-ups failed to meet the threshold criteria. The 2 other dogs displayed anxious behaviors such as avoiding treats and seeking the door. The performance outcomes of each hearing group for each cognitive test are provided in Table 3. Performance on the pointing cue and working memory tasks was not significantly different among the hearing groups but a significant decrease in performance was observed on inhibitory control (P = .001), detour (P = .01), and sustained gaze (P = .02) tasks as hearing deteriorated (Table 3).
Thirty-five of 39 dogs passed the warm-up criteria for cylinder tasks. Of the 4 dogs that failed, 2 were in the 50 dB group, 1 was in the 70 dB group, and 1 was in the 90 dB group. One of the dogs in the 50 dB group was too anxious to approach the unfamiliar cylinder despite repeated familiarization trials; the remaining dogs could not understand the objective and did not retrieve the treat during familiarization trials. All dogs completed the sustained gaze task. Pairwise comparisons among groups further emphasized the deterioration in task performance among groups for inhibitory control (Figure 4A), detour (Figure 4B), and sustained gaze (Figure 5).


3.6 Relationships among hearing threshold, age, CORQ, CADES, and cognitive testing
Multivariate analysis was performed for CORQ vitality and companionship scores, CADES scores, inhibitory control, and detour performance, with age and hearing groups as covariates (Table 4). Higher CADES score was significantly associated with worse hearing (P = .01, f2 = 1.5). Higher CADES score, worse CORQ domain scores, and worse inhibitory control performance all were associated with higher age (Table 4).
CORQ vitality | CORQ comp | CADES score | Inhibitory control | Detour | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hearing group | P = .65 | P = .66 | P = .01 | P = .12 | P = .08 |
Age | P = .007 | P = .02 | P = .01 | P = .05 | P = .36 |
- Abbreviations: CADES, Canine Dementia Scale; CCDS, canine cognitive dysfunction syndrome; Comp, companionship; CORQ, Canine Owner-Reported Quality of Life.
4 DISCUSSION
We investigated the relationships among presbycusis, QoL, owner-quantified CCDS and cognitive testing performance in dogs. Similar to previous findings,11, 12, 16, 37 our study suggests that presbycusis is prevalent in aging companion dogs. Owner assessment of QoL in the domains of vitality and companionship were both significantly associated with hearing loss, as were CCDS (as determined by CADES) and age. Specific executive control tasks were associated with hearing loss, whereas other tasks such as pointing cue (considered a social cue task) and working memory were not. Once the effect of age was taken into account in multivariate analysis, the significant association between CCDS severity and hearing loss was maintained.
Dog owners often are aware of changes in hearing as their pets age. Owners identified deficits in hearing in 75% of dogs in the 70 and 90 dB groups. By contrast, 83% of owners whose dogs tested in the 50 dB group believed their dogs' hearing was normal. However, the impact of presbycusis on QoL can be more challenging to assess. Quality of life in dogs is measured by a human proxy through interpretation of the pet's behavior. Our results show that scores of the vitality and companionship domains were significantly lower in dogs with hearing loss. These findings suggest that the quality of interactions between dogs and their owners declines as dogs' hearing declines. This change in perceived QoL is similar to findings in people with presbycusis. Moreover, the use of hearing aids in people experiencing presbycusis produces significant improvements in QoL.7, 9, 10, 38 In the absence of using hearing aids to assess improvements in QoL, a longitudinal study to track changes in hearing and owner-assessed QoL domains is warranted.
Dogs have been shown to respond differently based on tones of human voices39, 40 and our findings suggest that the interaction between dogs and their owners is affected by hearing loss. However, dogs in all 3 hearing categories performed similarly on the pointing task, which is a test of social cue recognition and does not require hearing. It is possible that dogs' use of visual cues may mitigate some of the impact of hearing loss, but owners did perceive a reduction in QoL regarding companionship in dogs with hearing loss, suggesting a similar impact on social functioning as encountered by people with hearing loss.41
We found a significant association between hearing loss and severity of CCDS as defined by CADES scoring in both univariate and multivariate analyses. The risk factors for CCDS currently are not well established. In contrast, numerous risk factors for dementia have been established in people including, but not limited to, hearing loss, level of education, hypertension, excessive alcohol consumption, obesity, smoking, depression, and systemic disease (ie, diabetes).3 The only potential risk factors previously identified in dogs for developing CCDS as assessed by CADES were aging and eating an uncontrolled diet (table scraps, mixture of diets, or low-quality commercial food), whereas other factors such as weight, sex, reproductive status, and housing type were not significantly associated with cognitive impairment.42 Another study, which used the canine cognitive dysfunction rating scale to assess for cognitive impairment in dogs >7 years old, did not find any significant differences in normal dogs and dogs with CCDS with factors such as age, breed, sex, body weight, reproductive status, nutrition, and medications.43 We believe that presbycusis could be a newly identified risk factor in dogs for development of CCDS based on CADES scoring and reduction in executive function test performance. However, we recognize that there may be an inherent bias in owner-reported CADES for dogs with hearing loss because some behaviors observed at home may be solely caused by presbycusis rather than representing true cognitive dysfunction. Unfortunately, without the use of hearing aids in dogs with presbycusis to observe for any immediate changes in behavior, it may be very difficult to determine the degree of owner reported bias when filling out the CADES questionnaire. Therefore, future studies should track changes in hearing and CADES longitudinally to investigate this relationship further.
The CADES questionnaire captures observations of owners on behavioral changes in their dogs at home, but does not quantify performance in specific cognitive domains. To determine which cognitive domains might be affected by hearing loss, we performed testing of executive function, response to social cues and working memory with these dogs in our laboratory. One cognitive domain impacted by age-related hearing loss in people is proficiency on working memory tests, especially those administered by verbal stimuli.6, 44 The use of hearing aids improves performance on memory tasks and slows decline in memory task performance for tests that rely on verbal stimuli.8, 45 In contrast, we did not find a significant difference between performance on working memory and hearing ability. One explanation is that our testing relied almost entirely on visual stimuli to test for working memory. Performance on short term memory tests that rely on visual stimuli in people are not always associated with degree of hearing loss.46-48 The differences between outcomes using visual and auditory focused working memory tests might be explained by the cognitive load hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, people with hearing loss devote more cognitive resources to understand speech, which in turn diverts available resources away from other processes such as a working memory task.49 Therefore, tests that rely on hearing for memory testing may be more affected by presbycusis than tests that rely on visual changes. Studies with working memory tests that use different forms of stimuli may help elucidate how presbycusis affects memory in dogs.
Executive function testing by means of the cylinder task (impulse control and detour tasks) disclosed significant differences among hearing groups and performance in univariate analysis. In pairwise analysis, a significant change in performance was found for dogs that fell in the 50 dB category and those that did not. This observation may indicate that a threshold in hearing loss exists that is associated with poorer performance on executive function tasks. In multivariate analyses with age as a covariate, age was the only variable significantly associated with inhibitory control performance, suggesting that a larger population of participants is required to explore the relationships among executive control, aging, and hearing in dogs further. Data about the relationship between peripheral presbycusis and executive control is mixed in humans. Some reports indicate that hearing loss is not associated with decline in executive functions.48, 50 People with hearing aids, however, experience improvements in executive functioning tasks.6, 8
Poorer hearing was associated with worse performance on another test of executive function, the sustained attention task, although this association was not significant on a multivariate analysis. This finding mirrors observations in humans in which cognitive decline is associated with poor performance on attention tasks.50 One confounding factor for performance on the sustained attention task is the influence of external noises on the ability to maintain concentration. Although trials with obvious distractions were excluded, the room in which testing was performed could not be completely sound proofed. In this instance, presbycusis could mitigate distractions and potentially improve performance on this test. Despite this, the dogs with hearing loss performed worse, and the potential relationship between attention and hearing loss deserves further evaluation in a larger population of dogs. The results of our cognitive testing data suggest that using different strategies for interactions at home (such as visual or physical cues) may help dogs with hearing loss in slowing their cognitive decline and this possibility should be further investigated in future studies.
Our study had some limitations. Firstly, our study was cross-sectional in nature. The role of hearing loss in development of cognitive decline in dogs should be investigated in a longitudinal study to track for changes in hearing and observe whether these changes occur before, concurrent with, or after cognitive decline. Furthermore, the effect of hearing loss on cognition in people can be evaluated readily by use of hearing aids. Middle ear implants have been used experimentally in 3 aged dogs with presbycusis and did improve their hearing.51 However, such implants are not readily available, and hearing aids for dogs are not used commonly, in part because they are poorly tolerated. This is likely to change in the future and the introduction of well-tolerated, affordable hearing aids for dogs will enhance our understanding of the effects of presbycusis on cognition and QoL in dogs.
Second, our study assessed the integrity of the peripheral and brainstem structures in the hearing pathway, but not cortical perception. Increasing evidence in humans indicates that processing of speech can be affected not only by peripheral presbycusis, which is assessed by BAER, but also by central presbycusis. A recent task force defined central presbycusis as age-related changes in the auditory portions of the central nervous system negatively impacting auditory perception, speech communication performance, or both.52 Central presbycusis also may increase the risk of development of Alzheimer's disease and progression of dementia.53 A reliable test of the central pathways that mediate a conscious response to auditory stimuli in pet dogs is needed. Although late BAER waveforms represent pathways projecting to the thalamus and cortex, they are difficult to quantify reliably. We recorded owner assessments of their dogs' hearing with the hope it might provide insight into central presbycusis when paired with BAER results. A subset of owners of dogs in the 50 dB group felt their dogs had a hearing problem without any evidence of presbycusis on BAER testing. Common complaints by these owners included failure of the dog to respond to its name, especially when outdoors, which often can be considered a noisy environment. Further work on the relationship between cognitive function and failure to respond to commands in the absence of diminished BAER is required for evaluation of central presbycusis in dogs.
Another limitation was the constraints of working with senior and geriatric companion dogs. A primary concern of the study was to obtain useful hearing tests while avoiding the risks of heavy sedation or anesthesia in these dogs. Dogs only received the minimum amount of sedation required for completion of the BAER test and would only tolerate testing 2 or 3 times. Hence, dogs were only tested at 3 different dB levels, using a click rather than specific tones. Future studies may benefit from correlating cognitive testing and QoL with a complete audiogram. Additionally, analysis of the BAER tracings was simplified to ensure consistency categorizing by the presence or absence of wave V in a minimum of 1 ear. Subtle changes in auditory evoked potentials exist such as changes in latency and amplitude. In humans, although the latency of wave V increased insignificantly with aging, the amplitudes of all waves were substantially decreased.54 Dogs of different breeds, however, have very different head and ear canal sizes and shapes. Our study featured a wide distribution of breeds and sizes of dogs. Some studies have identified a significant relationship between latency and amplitudes of certain waves and the size of the dog's head.55, 56 Fortunately, most of our test subjects have been enrolled in a longitudinal study, and the dogs will have repeated BAER examinations approximately every 6 months. Changes in latency and amplitude will be tracked over time for each dog and for each ear to improve our analysis. Finally, we used a QoL questionnaire developed for cancer patients that has not been validated for our population of dogs. A single question was specifically related to cancer and asked whether treatment had any effect on the pet's enjoyment of life. Because anxiety is commonly seen in dogs with CCDS and may impact life, we felt this substitution was reasonable. Otherwise, the questionnaire evaluated changes in domains (vitality, companionship, pain, and mobility) that are extremely relevant to changes associated with aging and therefore we felt it was reasonable to use it in our study.
In summary, aging is associated with hearing loss in companion dogs. Much like people who experience social isolation and depression with presbycusis, dog owners perceive a difference in particular aspects of their dog's QoL. Our data found that hearing loss was associated with severity of cognitive dysfunction as scored by CADES as well as poorer performance on certain cognitive tests of executive function. Hearing loss may be a field in which intervention could slow the progression of CCDS and therefore the relationships among presbycusis, aging, and dementia deserve further investigation in dogs.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Supported by the Dr. Kady M. Gjessing and Rhanna M. Davidson Distinguished Chair of Gerontology.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST DECLARATION
Authors declare no conflict of interest.
OFF-LABEL ANTIMICROBIAL DECLARATION
Authors declare no off-label use of antimicrobials.
INSTITUTIONAL ANIMAL CARE AND USE COMMITTEE (IACUC) OR OTHER APPROVAL DECLARATION
Approved by the North Carolina State University IACUC, protocol number 18-109-O.
HUMAN ETHICS APPROVAL DECLARATION
Authors declare human ethics approval was not needed for this study.