Naturally occurring circadian rhythm and sleep duration are related to executive functions in early adulthood
Corresponding Author
Liisa Kuula
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Correspondence
Liisa Kuula, MA, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Haartmaninkatu 3 (PO Box 21), 00014 Helsinki, Finland.
Tel.: +358294129401; fax: 02941 22527.
e-mail: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorAnu-Katriina Pesonen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorKati Heinonen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorEero Kajantie
National Institute for Health and Welfare, Chronic Disease Prevention Unit, Helsinki and Oulu, Finland
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
PEDEGO Research Unit, MRC Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorJohan Gunnar Eriksson
National Institute for Health and Welfare, Chronic Disease Prevention Unit, Helsinki and Oulu, Finland
Department of General Practice and Primary Health Care, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorSture Andersson
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorAulikki Lano
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorJari Lahti
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorDieter Wolke
Department of Psychology and Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, WMS, University of Warwick, Warwick, UK
Search for more papers by this authorKatri Räikkönen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Liisa Kuula
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Correspondence
Liisa Kuula, MA, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Haartmaninkatu 3 (PO Box 21), 00014 Helsinki, Finland.
Tel.: +358294129401; fax: 02941 22527.
e-mail: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorAnu-Katriina Pesonen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorKati Heinonen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorEero Kajantie
National Institute for Health and Welfare, Chronic Disease Prevention Unit, Helsinki and Oulu, Finland
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
PEDEGO Research Unit, MRC Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorJohan Gunnar Eriksson
National Institute for Health and Welfare, Chronic Disease Prevention Unit, Helsinki and Oulu, Finland
Department of General Practice and Primary Health Care, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Folkhälsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorSture Andersson
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorAulikki Lano
Children′s Hospital, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorJari Lahti
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorDieter Wolke
Department of Psychology and Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, WMS, University of Warwick, Warwick, UK
Search for more papers by this authorKatri Räikkönen
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Search for more papers by this authorSummary
Experimental sleep deprivation studies suggest that insufficient sleep and circadian misalignment associates with poorer executive function. It is not known whether this association translates to naturally occurring sleep patterns. A total of 512 of full-term-born members of the Arvo Ylppö Longitudinal Study [mean age = 25.3, standard deviation (SD) = 0.65] (44.3% men) wore actigraphs to define sleep duration, its irregularity and circadian rhythm (sleep mid-point) during a 1-week period (mean 6.9 nights, SD = 1.7). Performance-based executive function was assessed with the Trail-Making Test, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test and Stroop. The self-rated adult version of Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function was used to assess trait-like executive function. We found that performance-based and self-reported trait-like executive function correlated only modestly (all correlations ≤0.17). Shorter sleep duration associated with more commission errors. Later circadian rhythm associated with poorer trait-like executive function, as indicated by the Brief Metacognitive Index and the Behavior Regulation Index. Those belonging to the group with the most irregular sleep duration performed slower than others in the Trail-Making Test Part A. All associations were adjusted for sex, age, socioeconomic status and body mass index. In conclusion, naturally occurring insufficient sleep and later circadian rhythm showed modest associations with poorer executive function. Shorter habitual sleep duration was associated with lower scores of performance-based tests of executive function, and later circadian rhythm was associated mainly with poorer trait-like executive function characteristics. Our findings suggest additionally that sleep duration and circadian rhythm associate with different domains of executive function, and there are no additive effects between the two.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
jsr12581-sup-0001-TableS1.docxWord document, 27.9 KB | Table S1. Associations between sleep measurements and T-scores assessing executive function. |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- Banks, S. and Dinges, D. F. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J. Clin. Sleep Med., 2007, 3: 519–528.
- Bin, Y. S., Marshall, N. S. and Glozier, N. Sleeping at the limits: the changing prevalence of short and long sleep durations in 10 countries. Am. J. Epidemiol., 2013, 177: 826–833.
- Bjorkqvist, J., Paavonen, J., Andersson, S. et al. Advanced sleep-wake rhythm in adults born prematurely: confirmation by actigraphy-based assessment in the Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults. Sleep Med., 2014, 15: 1101–1106.
- Bohnen, N., Jolles, J. and Twijnstra, A. Modification of the Stroop color word test improves differentiation between patients with mild head injury and matched controls. Clin. Neuropsychol., 1992, 6: 178–184.
- Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B. and Ulrich, R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiol. Int., 2012, 29: 55–61.
- Cain, S. W., Silva, E. J., Chang, A. M., Ronda, J. M. and Duffy, J. F. One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain Cogn., 2011, 76: 37–42.
- Conners, C. K. The Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT II). Multi Health Systems, Toronto, Canada, 2004.
- Cotrena, C., Branco, L. D., Shansis, F. M. and Fonseca, R. P. Executive function impairments in depression and bipolar disorder: association with functional impairment and quality of life. J. Affect. Disord., 2016, 190: 744–753.
- Day, A. M., Kahler, C. W., Ahern, D. C. and Clark, U. S. Executive functioning in alcohol use studies: a brief review of findings and challenges in assessment. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev., 2015, 8: 26–40.
- De Souza, C. M. and Hidalgo, M. P. The midpoint of sleep on working days: a measure for chronodisruption and its association to individuals’ well-being. Chronobiol. Int., 2015, 32: 341–348.
- Digdon, N. L. and Howell, A. J. College students who have an eveningness preference report lower self-control and greater procrastination. Chronobiol. Int., 2008, 25: 1029–1046.
- Dixit, A. and Mittal, T. Executive functions are not affected by 24 hours of sleep deprivation: a color-word Stroop Task study. Indian J. Psychol Med., 2015, 37: 165–168.
- Duffy, J. F., Zitting, K. M. and Chinoy, E. D. Aging and Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Med. Clin., 2015, 10: 423–434.
- Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Altamirano, L. J. et al. Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal twin study. Dev. Psychol., 2016, 52: 326–340.
- Gobin, C. M., Banks, J. B., Fins, A. I. and Tartar, J. L. Poor sleep quality is associated with a negative cognitive bias and decreased sustained attention. J. Sleep Res., 2015, 24: 535–542.
- Hahn, C., Cowell, J. M., Wiprzycka, U. J. et al. Circadian rhythms in executive function during the transition to adolescence: the effect of synchrony between chronotype and time of day. Dev. Sci., 2012, 15: 408–416.
- Heinonen, K., Raikkonen, K., Pesonen, A. K. et al. Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age: a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics, 2008, 121: e1325–e1333.
- Jackson, M. L., Gunzelmann, G., Whitney, P. et al. Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev., 2013, 17: 215–225.
- Juda, M., Vetter, C. and Roenneberg, T. Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers. J. Biol. Rhythms, 2013, 28: 141–151.
- Lemola, S., Ledermann, T. and Friedman, E. M. Variability of sleep duration is related to subjective sleep quality and subjective well-being: an actigraphy study. PLoS ONE, 2013, 8: e71292.
- Lo, J. C., Groeger, J. A., Santhi, N. et al. Effects of partial and acute total sleep deprivation on performance across cognitive domains, individuals and circadian phase. PLoS ONE, 2012, 7: e45987.
- Moeller, S. J., Honorio, J., Tomasi, D. et al. Methylphenidate enhances executive function and optimizes prefrontal function in both health and cocaine addiction. Cereb. Cortex (NY), 2014, 24: 643–653.
- Pesonen, A. K., Martikainen, S., Heinonen, K. et al. Continuity and change in poor sleep from childhood to early adolescence. Sleep, 2014, 37: 289–297.
- Pyhala, R., Lahti, J., Heinonen, K. et al. Neurocognitive abilities in young adults with very low birth weight. Neurology, 2011, 77: 2052–2060.
- Reitan, R. M. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills, 1958, 8: 271–276.
10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271 Google Scholar
- Riegel, K., Ohrt, B., Wolke, D. and Österlund, K. Die Entwicklung Gefährdet Geborener Kinder bis zum Fünften Lebensjahr. [The development of children born at risk until their fifth year of life]Fernind Enke Verlag, Stuttgart, 1995.
- Rossa, K. R., Smith, S. S., Allan, A. C. and Sullivan, K. A. The effects of sleep restriction on executive inhibitory control and affect in young adults. J. Adolesc. Health, 2014, 55: 287–292.
- Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K. and Gioia, G. A. Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Adult Version. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Lutz, FL, 2005.
- Rowe, M., Mccrae, C., Campbell, J. et al. Actigraphy in older adults: comparison of means and variability of three different aggregates of measurement. Behav. Sleep Med., 2008, 6: 127–145.
- Rutters, F., Lemmens, S. G., Adam, T. C. et al. Is social jetlag associated with an adverse endocrine, behavioral, and cardiovascular risk profile? J. Biol. Rhythms, 2014, 29: 377–383.
- Salthouse, T. A. What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance? Intelligence, 2011, 39: 222–232.
- Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A. and Yonelinas, A. P. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: a meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 2016, 68: 651–668.
- Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F. and Boone, A. L. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers., 2004, 72: 271–324.
- Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. and Stanovich, K. E. Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 2013, 54: 131–143.
- Vartanian, O., Bouak, F., Caldwell, J. L. et al. The effects of a single night of sleep deprivation on fluency and prefrontal cortex function during divergent thinking. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2014, 8: 214.
- Whiting, W. L. and Murdock, K. K. Emerging adults’ sleep patterns and attentional capture: the pivotal role of consistency. Cogn. Process., 2016, 17: 155–162.
- Wilckens, K. A., Woo, S. G., Erickson, K. I. and Wheeler, M. E. Sleep continuity and total sleep time are associated with task-switching and preparation in young and older adults. J. Sleep Res., 2014, 23: 508–516.
- Wittmann, M., Dinich, J., Merrow, M. and Roenneberg, T. Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol. Int., 2006, 23: 497–509.
- Wolke, D., Sohne, B., Riegel, K., Ohrt, B. and Osterlund, K. An epidemiologic longitudinal study of sleeping problems and feeding experience of preterm and term children in southern Finland: comparison with a southern German population sample. J. Pediatr., 1998, 133: 224–231.
- Wong, P. M., Hasler, B. P., Kamarck, T. W., Muldoon, M. F. and Manuck, S. B. Social jetlag, chronotype, and cardiometabolic risk. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 2015, 100: 4612–4620.