When slowing down processing helps learning: Lexico-semantic structure supports retention, but interferes with disambiguation of novel object-label mappings
Corresponding Author
Arielle Borovsky
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Correspondence
Arielle Borovsky, Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA 47906.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Arielle Borovsky
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Correspondence
Arielle Borovsky, Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA 47906.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This project explores how children disambiguate and retain novel object-label mappings in the face of semantic similarity. Burgeoning evidence suggests that semantic structure in the developing lexicon promotes word learning in ostensive contexts, whereas other findings indicate that semantic similarity interferes with and temporarily slows familiar word recognition. This project explores how these distinct processes interact when mapping and retaining labels for novel objects (i.e., low-frequency objects that are unfamiliar to toddlers) via disambiguation from a semantically similar familiar referent in 24-month-olds (N = 65). Toddlers’ log-adjusted looking to labeled target objects (relative to distractor objects) was measured in three conditions: Familiar trials (familiar label spoken while viewing semantically related familiar and novel objects), Disambiguation trials (unfamiliar label spoken while viewing semantically similar familiar and unfamiliar object), and Retention trials (unfamiliar label spoken while viewing novel object pairs). Toddlers’ individual vocabulary structure was then compared to performance on each condition. Vocabulary structure was measured at two levels: category-level structure (semantic density) for experimental items, and lexicon-level structure (global clustering coefficient). The findings suggest, consistent with prior results, that semantic density interfered with known word recognition, and facilitated unfamiliar word retention. Children did not show a significant novel word preference during disambiguation, and disambiguation behavior was not impacted by semantic structure. These findings connect seemingly disparate mechanisms of semantic interference in processing and semantic leveraging in word learning. Semantic interference momentarily slows word recognition and resolution of referential uncertainty for novel label-object mappings. Nevertheless, this slowing might support retention by enabling comparison between related objects.
Open Research
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The dataset and analytic code that support the findings of this study are available at: https://osf.io/tqycs/?view_only=a572a3d5fb904d049a394a835b38a3ed
REFERENCES
- Arai, M., van Gompel, R. P. G., & Scheepers, C. (2007). Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54(3), 218–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.07.001
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
- Beckage, N., Smith, L., & Hills, T. (2011). Small worlds and semantic network growth in typical and late talkers. PLoS ONE, 6(5), e19348. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019348
- Bergelson, E., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Nature and origins of the lexicon in 6-mo-olds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(49), 12916–12921. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1712966114
- Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253–3258.
- Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months. Cognition, 126(1), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008
- Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://mitpress.mit.edu/books/how-children-learn-meanings-words
10.7551/mitpress/3577.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2012). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Retrieved from:http://www.praat.org/
- Borovsky, A., Ellis, E. M. E. M., Evans, J. L., & Elman, J. L. J. L. (2016a). Lexical leverage: Category knowledge boosts real-time novel word recognition in two-year-olds. Developmental Science, 19(6), 918–932. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12343
- Borovsky, A., Ellis, E. M., Evans, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (2016b). Semantic structure in vocabulary knowledge interacts with lexical and sentence processing in infancy. Child Development, 87(6), 1893–1908. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12554
- Borovsky, A., & Peters, R. E. (2019). Vocabulary size and structure affects real-time lexical recognition in 18-month-olds. PLoS ONE, 14(7), e0219290. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219290
- Chen, Q., & Mirman, D. (2012). Competition and cooperation among similar representations: Toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychological Review, 119(2), 417–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027175
- Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e62. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1500031X
- Clerkin, E. M., Hart, E., Rehg, J. M., Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2017). Real-world visual statistics and infants’ first-learned object names. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1711), https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0055
- Colunga, E., & Sims, C. E. (2017). Not only size matters: Early-talker and late-talker vocabularies support different word-learning biases in babies and networks. Cognitive Science, 41, 73–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12409
- Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., Poltrock, S., & Floccia, C. (2015). A methodological investigation of the Intermodal Preferential Looking paradigm: Methods of analyses, picture selection and data rejection criteria. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 151–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.005
- Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual ( 2nd edn.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
- Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 98–116. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98
- Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. Sekerina, E. Fernandez & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Language Processing in Children. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
- Graham, S. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Baker, R. K. (1998). Infants’ disambiguation of novel object words. First Language, 18(53), 149–164. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379801805302
10.1177/014272379801805302 Google Scholar
- Grassmann, S., Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1200. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01200
- Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2011). Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: A critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology, 48(12), 1711–1725. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x
- Hadley, E. B., Dickinson, D. K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2019). Building semantic networks: The impact of a vocabulary intervention on preschoolers’ depth of word knowledge. Reading Research Quarterly, 54(1), 41–61. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.225
- Hills, T. T., Maouene, J., Riordan, B., & Smith, L. B. (2010). The associative structure of language: Contextual diversity in early word learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(3), 259–273.
- Hills, T. T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., & Smith, L. B. (2009). Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks: Preferential attachment or preferential acquisition? Psychological Science, 20(6), 729–739. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02365.x
- Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K. A., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An ermegentist coalistion model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in. Child Development, 65(3, Serial No., 262).
- Horst, J. S. (2013). Context and repetition in word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 149. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00149
- Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1393–1409. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3
- Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-Month-Old Infants. Infancy, 13(2), 128–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000701795598
- Horst, J. S., Samuelson, L. K., Kucker, S. C., & McMurray, B. (2011). What’s new? Children prefer novelty in referent selection. Cognition, 118(2), 234–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COGNITION.2010.10.015
- Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96(1), B23–B32.
- Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31–F39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.x
- Jørgensen, R. N., Dale, P. S., Bleses, D., & Fenson, L. (2010). CLEX: A cross-linguistic lexical norms database. Journal of Child Language, 37(2), 419–428. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909009544
- Kaefer, T., Pinkham, A. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2009). Taxonomic Organization Scaffolds Young Children’s Learning from Storybooks: A Design Experiment. Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness.
- Knoeferle, P., & Kreysa, H. (2012). Can speaker gaze modulate syntactic structuring and thematic role assignment during spoken sentence comprehension? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00538
- Kucker, S. C., McMurray, B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2015). Slowing down fast mapping: Redefining the dynamics of word learning. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 74–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12110
- Kucker, S. C., McMurray, B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2018). Too much of a good thing: How novelty biases and vocabulary influence known and novel referent selection in 18-month-old children and associative learning models. Cognitive Science, 42, 463–493. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12610
- Lee, J. (2011). Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(1), 69–92. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000299
- Lewis, M., Cristiano, V., Lake, B. M., Kwan, T., & Frank, M. C. (2019). The role of developmental change and linguistic experience in the mutual exclusivity effect. PsyArXiv Preprint, (2013). https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wsx3a.
- Marchman, V. A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21(02), 339–366. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009302
- Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024
- Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
- McMurray, B. (2016). Language at three timescales: The role of real-time processes in language development and evolution. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(2), 393–407. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12201
- McMurray, B., Horst, J. S. S., & Samuelson, L. K. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review, 119(4), 831. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029872
- Merriman, W. E., Bowman, L. L., & Macwhinney, B. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54(3), 1–129.
- Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1995). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category (N3C) principle by young children who have Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100(3), 231–243.
- Mirman, D., & Magnuson, J. S. (2009). Dynamics of activation of semantically similar concepts during spoken word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 37(7), 1026–1039. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.7.1026
- Neuman, S., Newman, E. H., & Dwyer, J. (2011). Educational effects of a vocabulary intervention on preschoolers word knowledge and conceptual development: A cluster-randomized trial. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(3), 249–272. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.46.3.3
- Newman, M. E. J., Moore, C., & Watts, D. J. (2000). Mean-field solution of the small-world network model. Physical Review Letters, 84(14), 3201–3204. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.3201
- Perry, L. K., Axelsson, E. L., & Horst, J. S. (2016). Learning what to remember: Vocabulary knowledge and children’s memory for object names and features. Infant and Child Development, 25(4), 247–258. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1933
- Peters, R. E., & Borovsky, A. (2019). Modeling early lexico-semantic network development: Perceptual features matter most. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(4), 763–782. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000596
- Peters, R. E., McRae, K., & Borovsky, A. (in prep.). Semantic feature norms for early-acquired nouns.
- Quine, W. V. O.,(1960). Word and object MIT press. Cambridge MA
- R Core Team, (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
- Sadeghi, Z., McClelland, J. L., & Hoffman, P. (2015). You shall know an object by the company it keeps: An investigation of semantic representations derived from object co-occurrence in visual scenes. Neuropsychologia, 76, 52–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.031
- Sailor, K. M. (2013). Is vocabulary growth influenced by the relations among words in a language learner’s vocabulary? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(5), 1657–1662. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032993
- Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 1558–1568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010
- Stella, M., Beckage, N. M., & Brede, M. (2017). Multiplex lexical networks reveal patterns in early word acquisition in children. Scientific Reports, 7, 46730. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46730
- Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29(1), 41–78. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2901_3
- Storkel, H. L., & Adlof, S. M. (2009). The effect of semantic set size on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 306–320.
- Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. E. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 639–650. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.639
- Vales, C., & Fisher, A. V. (2019). When stronger knowledge slows you down: Semantic relatedness predicts children’s co-activation of related items in a visual search paradigm. Cognitive Science, 43(6), e12746. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12746
- Von Holzen, K., & Mani, N. (2012). Language nonselective lexical access in bilingual toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 569–586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.001
- Wienholz, A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2019). Semantic processing of adjectives and nouns in American Sign Language: Effects of reference ambiguity and word order across development. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 1–18.
- Yee, E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Eye movements to pictures reveal transient semantic activation during spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(1), 1–14.