Contextual influence over deriving others' true beliefs using a relational triangulation perspective-taking protocol (RT-PTP-M1)
Corresponding Author
Paul M. Guinther
Portland Psychotherapy
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Paul Guinther, Western Psychological and Counseling Services, PC, 7455 SW Beveland St., Tigard, OR 97223. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Paul M. Guinther
Portland Psychotherapy
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Paul Guinther, Western Psychological and Counseling Services, PC, 7455 SW Beveland St., Tigard, OR 97223. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This paper introduces the relational triangulation framework as a functional contextual expansion of the established Relational Frame Theory (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001) account of perspective-taking. Initial support for the new framework is provided through data collected with a novel relational triangulation perspective-taking protocol configured in the present study to show contextual influence over deriving true belief in others following the direct training of a “seeing leads to knowing” repertoire (Leslie & Frith, 1988). Eight verbally competent adults were directly trained to make operant discriminations on a first set of target stimuli (i.e., the identities of three distinct figurines) and then directly trained to make contextually controlled deictic pointing responses to a second set of target stimuli (i.e., to the relative location of a target beacon according to the signaled spatial perspective of the self vs. two others). The test for derivation was whether the stimuli that had directly acquired contextual control over deictic perspective-taking during training would spontaneously exert contextual control over figurine discrimination relative to the spatial perspective of the two others. That is, passing the test for derivation required participants to infer that the others would “report what they were seeing” the same way that the self would if the self were in their position, suggesting coordination of the self and others. Seven of the eight participants exhibited the intended derivation of the others' “true beliefs,” confirming successful relational triangulation perspective-taking protocol configuration for this purpose.
References
- Baer, D. M., Peterson, R. F., & Sherman, J. A. (1967). The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10(5), 405-416.
- Bandura, A. (1969). Social learning theory of identificatory processes. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 213-262). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
- Barnes, D., Hegarty, N. & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Relating equivalence relations to equivalence relations: A relational framing model of complex human functioning. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 57-83.
- Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Cullinan, V. (2001). Education. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche, (Eds.) Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 181-196). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
- Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Smeets, P. M., Cullinan, V., & Leader, G. (2004). Relational frame theory and stimulus equivalence: Conceptual and procedural issues. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4(2), 181-214.
- Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme, 34, 174-183.
- Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46.
- Belisle, J., Dixon, M. R., Stanley, C. R., Munoz, B., & Daar, J. H. (2016). Teaching foundational perspective-taking skills to children with autism using the PEAK-T curriculum: Single-reversal “I–You” deictic frames. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4), 1-5.
- Brüne, M. (2005). “Theory of Mind” in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 21-42.
- Bühler, K. (1934). The deictic field of language and deictic words. Reprinted in R. Jarvella and W. Klein (Eds.), Speech, place and action ( 9-30). New York, NY: Wiley.
- Cairns, B. (1991). Spatial deixis: The use of spatial co-ordinates in spoken language. Lund University, Department of Linguistics, Working Papers, 38, 19-28.
- Campbell, J. (1994). Past, space, and self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
10.7551/mitpress/5262.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126.
- Davis, M. H., & Oathout, A. H. (1987). Maintenance of satisfaction in romantic relationships: Empathy and relational competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 397-410.
- Dennett, D. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Dickens, T. E., & Dickens, D. W. (2001). Symbols, stimulus equivalence, and the origins of language. Behavior and Philosophy, 29, 221-244.
- Diessel, H. (2006). Demonstratives, joint attention, and the emergence of grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(4), 463-489.
- Dougher, M. J., Hamilton, D. A., Fink, B. C., & Harrington, J. (2007). Transformation of the discriminative and eliciting functions of generalized relational stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(2), 179-197.
- Dvash, J., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., (2014). Theory of Mind and empathy as multidimensional constructs. Top Language Disorders, 34(4), 282-295.
- Foody, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Luciano, C. (2013). An empirical investigation of hierarchical versus distinction relations in a self-based ACT exercise. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 13(3), 373-388.
- Friedman, A. (2005). Examining egocentric and allocentric frames of reference in virtual space systems. Sprouts: Working Papers on Information Systems, 5(16), 50-63.
- Gil, E., Luciano, C., Ruiz, F. J., Valdivia-Salas, S. (2012). A preliminary demonstration of transformation of functions through hierarchical relations. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 12(1), 1-19.
- Gilroy, S. P., Lorah, E. R., Dodge, J., & Fiorello, C. (2015). Establishing deictic repertoires in autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 82-92.
- Greer, R. D., Dudek-Singer, J., & Gautreaux, G. (2006). Observational learning. International Journal of Psychology, 41(6), 486-499.
- Guinther, P. M., & Dougher, M. J. (2014). Partial contextual control of semantic false memories in the DRIFT paradigm. The Psychological Record, 4, 457-473.
- Hargreaves, S. (1998). Allegro 4 (version 4.4.2) [computer programming library]. Available from http://liballeg.org/license.html
- Harman, I. P. (1990). Teaching indirect speech: Deixis points the way. ELT Journal, 44(3), 230-238.
10.1093/elt/44.3.230 Google Scholar
- Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality. Behaviorism, 12, 99-110.
- S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.). (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
10.1007/b108413 Google Scholar
- Hayes, S. C., & Grundt, A. M. (1997). Metaphor, meaning and relational frame theory. In C. Mandell & A. McCabe (Eds.), The problem of meaning: behavioral and cognitive perspectives (pp. 117-146). Netherlands: Elsevian Science B.V.
10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80135-5 Google Scholar
- Hayes, S. C., & Sanford, B. T. (2014). Cooperation came first: Evolution and human cognition. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 112-129.
- Hayes S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford.
- Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching perspective-taking skills to typically developing children through derived relational responding. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 3(1), 1-34.
10.1037/h0100321 Google Scholar
- Jackson, M. L., Mendoza, D. R., & Adams, A. N. (2014). Teaching a deictic relational repertoire to children with autism. The Psychological Record, 64(4), 791-802.
- Kibrik, A. A., & Prozorova, E. V. (2007). Referential choice in signed and spoken languages. In Proceedings of 6th Discourse Anaphora and Anaphor Resolution Colloquium (pp. 41-46). Oporto, Portugal: Center for Linguistics of the University of Oporto.
- Klein, W. (1982). Local deixis in route directions. In R. J. Jarvella & W. Klein (Eds.), Speech, place, and action: Studies in deixis and related topics (pp. 161-182). New York, NY: Wiley.
- Lanius, R. A. (2015). Trauma-related dissociation and altered states of consciousness: A call for clinical, treatment, and neuroscience research. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 6, 27905. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.27905
- Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315-324.
- Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Crosslinguistic evidence. In P. Bloom, M.A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 109-169). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
- Levinson, S. C. (2004). Deixis. In The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 97-121). Oxford: Blackwell.
- Lovett, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2014). An evaluation of multiple exemplar instruction to teach perspective-taking skills to adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19(2), 22-36.
10.1037/h0100575 Google Scholar
- Luciano, C., Ruiz, F. J., Vizcaíno Torres, R. M., Sánchez Martín, V., Gutiérrez Martínez, O., López López, J. C. (2011). A relational frame analysis of defusion in acceptance and commitment therapy: A preliminary and quasi-experimental study with at-risk adolescents. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 11(2), 165-182.
- McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. Psychological Record, 54, 115-144.
- McLoughlin, S., & Stewart, I. (2017). Empirical advances in studying relational networks. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(3), 329-342.
- Ozonoff, S., & South, M. (2001). Early social development in young children with autism: Theoretical and clinical implications. In G. Bremner & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 565-588). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
- Peláez, M. (2009). Joint attention and social referencing in infancy as precursors of derived relational responding. In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.), Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities (pp. 63-78). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.
- Peláez, M., Virues-Ortega, J., & Gewirtz, J. L. (2012). Acquisition of social referencing via discrimination training in infants. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 23-35.
- Premack, D. G., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526.
- Rehfeldt, R., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M., & Kowalchuck, R. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47.
- Rendón, M. I., Soler, F., & Cortés, M. (2012). Simple deictic relations, perspective-taking and social competence. Suma Psicológica, 19(2), 19-37.
- Roche, B. & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily applicable relational responding and sexual categorization: A critical test of the derived difference relation. The Psychological Record, 46, 451-475.
- Rosenthal, T. L., & Bandura, A. (1978). Psychological modeling: Theory and practice. In S. L. Garfield and A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empirical analysis (pp. 621-658). New York, NY: Wiley.
- Schlinger, H. D. (2009). Theory of mind: An overview and behavioral perspective. The Psychological Record, 59, 435-448.
- Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Author's Cooperative.
- Sigman, M., & Capps, L. (1997). Children with autism: A developmental perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Smith, A. (2006). Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and cognition. The Psychological Record, 56, 3-21.
- Spradlin, J. E., & Brady, N. (2008). A behavior analytic interpretation of theory of mind. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8(3), 335-350.
- Stewart, I., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Understanding metaphor: A relational frame perspective. The Behavior Analyst, 24(2), 191-199.
- Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., Richeson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Perspective-taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1027-1042.
- Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85-101). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
- Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124-129.
- Vilardaga, R., Estévez, A., Levin, M. E., & Hayes, S. C. (2012). Deictic relational responding, empathy, and experiential avoidance as predictors of social anhedonia: Further contributions from relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 62, 409-432.
- Villatte, M., Monestès, J., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2008). Assessing deictic relational responding in social anhedonia: A functional approach to the development of theory of mind impairments. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4, 360-373.
10.1037/h0100867 Google Scholar
- Villatte, M., Monestès, J., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010). Assessing perspective-taking in schizophrenia using Relational Frame Theory. The Psychological Record, 60, 413-436.
- Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011). Establishing a deictic relational repertoire in young children. The Psychological Record, 61, 371-390.
- Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684.
- Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2006). The collateral effects of joint attention training on social initiations, positive affect, imitation, and spontaneous speech for young children with autsim. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 655-664.
- Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82(2), 177-195.