Automatic effects of deviancy cues on creative cognition
Corresponding Author
Jens Förster
International University Bremen, Germany
International University Bremen, Campus Ring 1, PO Box 750561, D-28725 Bremen, Germany.Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Jens Förster
International University Bremen, Germany
International University Bremen, Campus Ring 1, PO Box 750561, D-28725 Bremen, Germany.Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Three experiments test the existence of an automatic deviancy-creativity link. Using a lexical decision task, in Experiment 1 we found a semantic link between deviancy and creativity words in that decision times for creativity-related words were enhanced after subliminal deviancy priming. In Experiment 2, participants were led to think about either a punk or an engineer and afterwards were administered creative insight and analytical reasoning problems. According to a pretest, punks and engineers were judged as differing in uniqueness but not in creativity. Participants given ‘punk’ priming solved more creative insight problems and fewer analytical reasoning problems than those given ‘engineer’ priming. In Experiment 3, participants were incidentally exposed to abstract artworks symbolically expressing either the concept of conformity or deviancy and were subsequently asked to solve a creative generation task. Exposure to the artwork representing deviancy led to generation of more creative solutions than exposure to that representing conformity. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
REFERENCES
- Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63.
- Aarts, H., & Dijksterhius, A. (2003). The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 18–28.
- Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
- Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszcynski, T., & Schimel, J. (1999). Creativity and terror management: Evidence that creative activity increases guilt and social projection following mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 19–32.
- Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis, & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of the underlying: An automatic power–sex association and its consquences for sexual harassmnet and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 768–781.
- Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.
- Barron, F. (1955). The disposition towards originality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 478–485.
- Bruner, J. (1962). The conditions of creativity. In H. E. Gruber, G. Terrell, & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to creative thinking. New York: Atherton Press.
10.1037/13117-001 Google Scholar
- Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froming, W. J., & Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 403–421.
- Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer, & T. Srull (Eds.), The handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 323–418). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of trivial pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–877.
- Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., van Knippenberg, A., & Scheepers, D. (1998). Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 862–871.
- Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (1999). Just going along: Nonconscious priming and conformity to social pressure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 578–589.
- Eysenck, M. W. (1993). Principles of cognitive psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In C. Hendrick, & M. Clark (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology: Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 74–97). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
- Fiore, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (1998). Right hemisphere contributions to creative problem solving: Converging evidence for divergent thinking. In M. Beeman, & C. Chiarello (Eds.), Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 349–371). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory Focus and the ‘Goal Looms Larger’ effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131.
- Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2000). The effects of approach and avoidance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–492.
- Friedman, R., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. In Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013.
- Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2002). The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 41–55.
- Friedman, R., & Förster, J. (In press). Effects of motivational cues on perceptual asymmetry: Implications for creativity and analytical problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
- Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.
- Hebb, D. O. (1948). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
- Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1106–1115.
- Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability and salience. In E. T. Higgins, & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press.
- Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and Prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Psychology, 30, 1–46.
- Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141–154.
- Hilgard, E. R., & Bower, G. H. (1975). Theories of learning. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
- Hirt, E. R., McDonald, H. E., & Melton, R. J. (1996). Processing goals and the affect-performance link: Mood as main effect or mood as input? In L. L. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 303–328). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis, & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions ( 2nd ed., pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford.
- Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.
- McGraw, K. O. (1978). The detrimental effects of reward on performance: A literature review and a prediction model. In M. R. Lepper, & D. Greene (Eds.), The hidden costs of rewards. Hillsdale, NJ: Elbaum.
- Metcalfe, J., & Wiebe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 15, 238–246.
- Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. (1990). The influence of mood on categorization: A cognitive flexibility interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 411–425.
- Mussweiler, T., & Förster, J. (2000). The sex—>aggression link: A perception-behavior dissociation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 507–520.
- Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.
- Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner, & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Ohlsson, S. (1984). Restructuring revisited: II. An information processing theory of restructuring and insight. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 25, 117–129.
- Schooler, J. W., Ohlson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 166–183.
- Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Risky and careful processing under stereotype threat: How regulatory focus can enhance and deteriorate performance when self stereotypes are active. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38–56.
- Simonton, D. K. (1991). Emergence and realization of genius: The lives and works of 120 classical composers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 829–840.
- Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
- Sternberg, R. J. (1999). A propulsion model of types of creative contributions. Review of General Psychology, 3, 83–100.
10.1037/1089-2680.3.2.83 Google Scholar
- Sternberg, R. J. (2001). What is the Common Thread of Creativity? Its Dialectical Relation to Intelligence and Wisdom. American Psychologist, 56, 360–362.
- Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its development. Human Development, 34, 1–31.
- Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.
- Taylor, D. (1960). Thinking and creativity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 91, 108–127.
- Walther, E., Müller, D., & Schott, O. (2001). Automatisches soziales Verhalten. Wie wirkt sich die Aktivierung der Konzepte Altruismus und Egoismus auf Hilfeleistung aus? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 248–257.
- Wyer, R., Clore, G. L., & Isbell, L. (1999). Affect and information processing. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 31, pp. 1–77). New York: Academic Press.
10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60271-3 Google Scholar