Volume 31, Issue 4 pp. 262-274
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Open Access

Mental health-related police incidents: Results of a national census exercise in England and Wales

Eddie Kane

Corresponding Author

Eddie Kane

Centre for Health and Justice, Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK

Correspondence

Eddie Kane, Centre for Health and Justice, Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, Triumph Road, Nottingham, NG7 2TU, UK.

Email: [email protected] and [email protected]

Search for more papers by this author
Jack CattellJulia Wire

Julia Wire

College of Policing Ltd, London, UK

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 23 July 2021

Abstract

Background

The level of mental state incidents dealt with by police and the police resource involved is under-researched, often giving rise to un-evidenced claims around demand, response and resources. The 2019 National Police Chiefs' Council and College of Policing definition of such incidents provides a useful basis for more accurate calculation: ‘Any police incident thought to relate to someone's mental health where their vulnerability is at the centre of the incident or where the police have had to do something additionally or differently because of it’.

Aims

To establish the nature and frequency of incidents involving the police when mental state is a primary reason for the involvement.

Methods

In this mixed methods study, we first analysed data from records in two mixed inner city/urban/rural forces and one large multi-local authority metropolitan force. Secondly, we made an in-depth analysis of a sample of mental state-related incidents (n = 320) in two of these forces. Thirdly, we took a 24-hour snapshot of all such incidents in England and Wales.

Results

Mental state-related incidents accounted for 5.1% of recorded police contacts from the public, rising to just 7.8% when confining attention only to contacts that generated a police response beyond taking the call and recording it. Length of time between an incident being reported and first response was similar between mental state-related and non-mental state-related incidents, but response to closure time was shorter for the former.

Conclusions

While incidents relating to mental state problems do consume police resources, they do not represent disproportionate demand in terms of numbers or time spent. That said, only about a quarter of the police work recorded was related to possible crimes, and the possibility of conflating perception of wider social need with mental state problems may further account for an apparent mismatch between the perceived and actual proportion of the workload spent on these incidents.

1 INTRODUCTION

The police frequently fulfil the role of gatekeeper for people experiencing mental health problems. There is some evidence of criminalisation of the mentally ill if this policing role is not performed appropriately (Lamb et al., 2014). Other research has shown that police involvement with individuals suspected of minor offences and flagged as experiencing mental health problems are more likely to be arrested than those who are not. Indeed, for offences of equivalent severity, a person with mental state problems is twice as likely to be arrested as one without (Charette et al., 2014) and are more often charged and spend longer in police custody (Kane et al., 2018). This type of encounter may require officers to have specific training and skills (Lamb et al., 2014) and depend on the availability of hospital and community mental health resources for successful outcomes (Kane et al., 2018). The same individuals who have unresolved needs are likely to have repeated contacts. Occasionally, they may involve volatile situations, risking the safety of all involved (Reuland et al., 2012). In England and Wales, the police are given powers under section 136 (s136) Mental Health Act (MHA) 1983/2007 to remove a person from a public place, when they appear to be suffering from a mental disorder, to a place of safety to facilitate assessment of their health and well-being as well as the safety of other people around them (Thomas & Forrester-Jones, 2019).

Clearly, there are complexities facing police and individuals experiencing mental health problems when the two come into contact, whether the individuals are victims, suspects or witnesses. The fairly extensive related literature, however, has significant gaps and few high-quality studies (Kane et al., 2017). The level of police resource tied up by mental state-related incidents is a topic largely devoid of substantial, objective research. This has been complicated by the often inexact and idiosyncratic categorisation of mental state-related incidents by different police forces and key individuals within them. A mental state-related flag on a police Command and Control System (CCR) could have been generated in ways ranging from an inaccurate or out of date note on the system, by a call handler with little training, the caller himself/herself, or have involved a mental health professional or information from a healthcare record. Consequently, the accuracy of the flag varies a great deal, with a danger that such a mixed and un-systematised categorisation leaves too much room for un-evidenced claims and counter claims around demand, response and resources (Kane et al., 2017).

An important part of understanding the perceptions of the police in relation to individuals with mental state problems is how they construct their view of these problems and those experiencing them. There is an extensive literature on the impact of social construction on official data and its impact on how different groups and communities are perceived by organisations and their members (see Bor et al., 2018; Javaid, 2018; Sampson & Raudenbush, 2004). Social construction assumes not only that people create, make or invent their understandings of the world and the meanings of encounters with others but also that they do this jointly, in coordination with others rather than individually (Leeds-Hurwitz, 2016). How does this work for police in their work encounters with people with mental health problems?

This study was commissioned and carried out as a result of a decision by the National Police Chiefs' Council (NPCC) to develop a better understanding of mental state-related demand on police resources. This decision was prompted by concerns expressed about the impact of incidents relating to people with mental state problems on police resources by Her Majesty's Inspectorate of Constabulary Fire and Rescue (HMICFRS; Picking up the Pieces, 2018). Our study involved several elements (1) a national exercise using a 24-hour, national snapshot census carried out on 12 November 2019 in all forces in England and Wales (the date was chosen by the commissioners of the study); (2) an analysis of 12 months of more in-depth data from three case study forces ranging from large metropolitan, mixed urban and predominantly rural forces; and (3) a series of semi-structured interviews and focus groups with a wide range of warranted officers and civilian staff of all ranks in these and seven other forces.

The study was designed to capture, first, the number of incidents that met the criteria set out in a new NPCC and College of Policing (CoP) definition of what constitutes a mental state-related incident, developed after inspections by HMICFRS had identified that forces were using different definitions. The new definition was published in 2019. The definition states ‘Any police incident thought to relate to someone's mental health where their vulnerability is at the centre of the incident or where the police have had to do something additionally or differently because of it’ (NPCC & CoP, 2020).

The other main aim of the study was to understand the nature and impact of incidents meeting this definition.

2 METHODS

2.1 Ethics

The study was approved by the Problem Solving and Demand Reduction Programme and endorsed by the NPCC and CoP.

2.2 Research design

We used a mixed method design which gave us a richer and more complete picture by triangulating findings from the quantitative and qualitative arms of the study (Maruna, 2010); however, this article reports only on the findings of the quantitative arm, and a future article will report the qualitative arm. This allowed us to report the 24-hour snapshot which the NPCC and HMICFRS had requested and also to gain a more in-depth picture of mental state-related incidents and their impact on policing. The quantitative work involved two elements: (1) an in-depth analysis in three forces – two mixed inner city/urban/rural forces and one large multi-local authority metropolitan force and (2) a snapshot of mental state-related activity at all forces in England and Wales on 12 November 2019. This day (a Tuesday) was chosen by the funders. There is no historical evidence to suggest it was atypical.

2.3 Procedure and data collection

2.3.1 Snapshot

For the snapshot, we asked all forces in England and Wales to complete a series of data templates with information on mental state-related incidents that met the new definition for the 24 hours between 00:00 and 23:59 on 12 November 2019. As well as the data template, a set of instructions was shared, which described the sample period and defined the data variables. They were asked to include police recorded incidents (whether or not there was a police response), individuals taken into police custody and missing persons events. Forty-four police forces shared data (43 Home Office forces in England and Wales and British Transport Police).

Although the actual snapshot was for 12 November, the final data set was not complete until early 2020, because some incidents are not closed and fully recorded for weeks or even months. Data from one force were excluded because they had no recorded mental health-related incidents and were therefore considered an outlier. This meant that while most forces completed the data sheets in real-time some completed them retrospectively. A comparison of the differently timed data returns suggested no evidence of resulting systematic bias.

The forces provided their flags for mental state-related incidents (as described in the introduction) and were asked to review these retrospectively to determine whether the event met the official definition. Not all police forces took this latter step and, where not, the original records were used for the study.

All data returns were processed in R (R provides a wide variety of statistical [linear and nonlinear modelling, classical statistical tests, time-series analysis, classification and clustering] and graphical techniques).

2.3.2 Case study forces

For the in-depth work in the case study forces, data were collected between 1 August 2018 and 31 July 2019 on police recorded incidents (from force command and control systems), individuals taken into police custody (from custody records) and missing persons events (from a range of force management systems). We also carried out an in-depth case-by-case analysis of a sample of incident cases (n = 320) in two of the forces (one metropolitan and one mixed localities) to explore the extent to which closer case scrutiny might highlight issues over and above those in the snapshot.

Each force shared data extracts from their relevant systems. The data were cleaned and processed in R. The codes developed for the snapshot were also used to standardise the analysis. One case study force, however, was unable to share missing persons events.

2.4 Statistical analysis

All quantitative data were fully described using frequency tables and cross-tabulations. Regression analysis was used to estimate the time taken to respond to incidents controlling for relevant factors.

3 RESULTS

3.1 Prevalence of calls and calculated time commitment

Table 1 shows that of over 39,000 incidents recorded in the 24-hour snapshot, just over 2000 had been flagged as mental health-related. Only 5.1% of incidents were so recorded, but there was some variation between forces (1.4%, n = 18, to 17.7%, n = 29). Just over a quarter of the much smaller number of police custody cases were thought to have mental health problems. Nearly one third of missing persons had been flagged with possible mental state issues.

TABLE 1. MS incidents, custodies and missing persons events (snapshot data and three case study forces)
Event MS Non-MS Total
n % n % n %
Snapshot
Incidents 2139 5.1 39,493 94.9 41,632 100.0
Custodies 1046 25.7 3030 74.3 4076 100.0
Missing persons 263 30.4 603 69.6 866 100.0
Force study 1
Incidents 24,033 4.3 529,529 95.7 553,562 100.0
Custodies 215 0.8 26,523 99.2 26,738 100.0
Missing persons 351 3.2 10,725 96.8 11,076 100.0
Force study 2
Incidents 24,436 5.6 409,795 94.4 434,231 100.0
Custodies 8078 30.1 18,738 69.9 26,816 100.0
Missing persons 2051 26.8 5614 73.2 7665 100.0
Force study 3
Incidents 17,964 1.6 1,076,282 98.4 1,094,246 100.0
Custodies 12,223 23.5 39,807 76.5 52,030 100.0
Missing persons - - - - - -
  • Abbreviation: MS, mental state.

Table 2 shows the nature of responses to the calls – according to snapshot data and also from the more detailed three force data collection. When we looked specifically at the calls in the snapshot data eliciting a police response over and above the recording and disposal, only 7.8% were related to mental health (MH) incidents.

TABLE 2. Priority of incidents (snapshot data and three case studies)
Priority MS Non-MS
n % n %
Snapshot
Immediate/emergency 639 31.7 6837 17.9
Priority 634 31.4 7788 20.4
Standard 27 1.3 934 2.4
Appointment 134 6.6 5262 13.8
Resolved without deployment 505 25.0 14,482 37.9
Information 38 1.9 1186 3.1
Other 40 2.0 1390 3.6
Not known - - 328 0.9
Total 2017 99.9 38,207 100.0
Force study 1
Immediate/emergency 9219 38.4 103,526 19.6
Priority 6197 25.8 127,043 24.0
Standard - - - 0.0
Appointment 3296 13.7 126,483 23.9
Resolved without deployment 5090 21.2 154,201 29.1
Information - - - 0.0
Other 231 1.0 18,276 3.5
Total 24,033 100.1 529,529 100.1
Force study 2
Immediate/emergency 9674 39.6 89,444 21.8
Priority 5046 20.6 60,987 14.9
Standard 1575 6.4 38,215 9.3
Appointment 1001 4.1 44,862 10.9
Resolved without deployment 6540 26.8 161,676 39.5
Information - - - -
Other 600 2.5 14,595 3.6
Not known - - - -
Total 24,436 100.0 409,779 100.0
Force study 3
Immediate/emergency 8843 49.2 211,548 19.7
Priority 3776 21.0 169,612 15.8
Standard 1 0.0 420 0.0
Appointment 533 3.0 73,840 6.9
Resolved without deployment 3979 22.1 527,268 49.0
Information - - - -
Other 832 4.6 93,594 8.7
Not known - - - -
Total 17,964 99.9 1,076,282 100.1
  • Abbreviation: MS, mental state.

We also analysed the opening and closing codes of incidents in both the snapshot and the three force studies (Tables 3 and 4). We found, with one exception, that the majority of incidents were opened and closed as ‘public safety and welfare’. The exception was in Force 3, where ‘other’ and ‘public safety and welfare’ were the majority opening code and the majority closing code, respectively. This means that most of the police involvement in mental state-related incidents was not designated as crime but more connected to the vulnerability of the individuals and that both snapshot recordings and more in-depth selected force data were consistent.

TABLE 3. Incident opening code (snapshot data and three case studies)
Priority  MS Non-MS
n % n %
Snapshot
Administration 188 8.8 5883 15.0
Antisocial behaviour 124 5.8 4679 11.9
Crime 224 10.5 9994 25.4
No offence - - 2 0.0
Not applicable - - 43 0.1
Not recorded 7 0.3 189 0.5
Other 9 0.4 367 0.9
Public safety 1583 74.0 12,942 32.9
Transport 4 0.2 5204 13.2
Total 2139 100.00 39,303 99.9
Force study 1
Administration 3464 14.4 73,075 13.8
Antisocial behaviour 1813 7.5 52,503 9.9
Crime 2225 9.3 114,243 21.6
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - - -
Other 10 0.0 495 0.1
Public safety 16,424 68.3 207,292 39.1
Transport 97 0.4 81,919 15.5
Total 24,033 99.9 529,527 100.00
Force study 2
Administration 3088 12.6 59,589 14.5
Antisocial behaviour 511 2.1 31,591 7.7
Crime 2990 12.2 106,847 26.1
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - - -
Other 2 0.0 626 0.2
Public safety 17,784 72.8 152,050 37.1
Transport 61 0.2 59,070 14.4
Total 24,436 99.9 409,773 100.0
Force study 3
Administration - - - -
Antisocial behaviour 1902 10.6 79,233 7.4
Crime 846 4.7 148,522 13.8
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - - -
Other 9278 51.6 602,976 56.0
Public safety 5853 32.6 141,453 13.1
Transport 85 0.5 104,098 9.7
Total 17,964 100.0 1,076,282 100.0
  • Abbreviation: MS, mental state.
TABLE 4. Incident closing code (snapshot data and three case studies)
Priority MS Non-MS
n % n %
Snapshot
Administration 246 12.5 7868 22.2
Antisocial behaviour 42 2.1 2381 6.7
Crime 185 9.4 8770 24.7
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - 44 0.1
Not recorded - - - -
Other 6 0.3 292 0.8
Public safety 1485 75.4 12,278 34.6
Transport 6 0.3 3861 10.9
Total 1970 100.0 35,494 100.0
Force study 1
Administration 2871 11.9 147,387 27.8
Antisocial behaviour 628 2.6 33,053 6.2
Crime 3034 12.6 120,687 22.8
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - 2660 0.5
Other - - - -
Public safety 17,452 72.6 156,528 29.6
Transport 48 0.2 69,214 13.1
Total 24,033 99.9 529,529 100.0
Force study 2
Administration - - - -
Antisocial behaviour 656 2.7 33,892 8.3
Crime 2918 11.9 110,041 26.9
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - - -
Other 3313 13.6 74,955 18.3
Public safety 17,502 71.6 136,422 33.3
Transport 44 0.2 54,452 13.3
Total 24,433 100.0 409,762 100.1
Force study 3
Administration 600 3.3 265,265 24.7
Antisocial behaviour 230 1.3 55,797 5.2
Crime 497 2.8 234,445 21.8
No offence - - - -
Not applicable - - - -
Not recorded - - - -
Other - - 165 0.0
Public safety 16,623 92.5 434,298 40.4
Transport 14 0.1 84,898 7.9
Total 17,964 100.0 1,074,868 100.0
  • Abbreviation: MS, mental state.

Finally, we looked at the time to police closure of incidents, including those incidents for which police used powers under section 136 of the MHA 1983/2007 (Table 5). We found that the length of time between an incident being dispatched and cleared varied little between mental state-related and non-mental state related incidents. Notably, given the anecdotal evidence we collected on delays associated with mental health-related incidents, according to both snapshot and three force data, time between notification and first response was similar for mental health- and non-mental health-related cases, but substantially shorter, on average, between first response and closure for the mental health-related cases.

TABLE 5. Time to close and dispose of incidents (snapshot data and three force studies)
Average time (hours) MS Non-MS S136
Mean SD n Mean SD n Mean SD n
Snapshot
Between despatch and clear 2.9 3.7 1114 2.2 3.4 11,296 5.6 4.9 65
Between clear and dispose 4.1 12.4 806 5.6 15.4 8733 3.1 8.8 49
Force study 1
Between despatch and clear 4.7 11.6 13,320 4.7 15.2 202,551 - - -
Between clear and dispose 9.6 22.5 160,413 16.5 30.4 12,496 - - -
Force study 2
Between despatch and clear 3.1 8.0 14,899 2.8 11.9 163,137 - - -
Between clear and dispose 2.9 8.0 4729 10.6 17.8 18 - - -
Force study 3
Between despatch and clear 2.3 1.9 3063 2.0 1.8 94,886 - - -
Between clear and dispose 5.4 15.1 2208 9.7 23.8 68,124 - - -

4 DISCUSSION

It seems clear from both the snapshot and the extended data from the case studies that we analysed, that actual mental state-related incidents are significantly fewer than the anecdotal evidence suggested. The data from the 24-hour national snapshot, the case studies in three very different forces and the detailed analysis of 320 cases, indicate that while mental state-related incidents do consume police resources, some taking more time than others and can be stressful and complex to deal with, both for the police and for the individuals involved, they do not represent a disproportionate amount of demand on police time and resources compared to non-mental state-related incidents. However, the data we collected does indicate that only about a quarter of what the police dealt with were crimes. This in itself may amplify their feeling that they spend a lot of their time dealing with social issues, including those related to mental state problems.

There is a relatively commonly held view that mental state tags/flags in police systems under-record mental state incidents; however, the in-depth work we did as part of this project indicates that even when a sample of 320 mental state-related incident cases was analysed in detail, no significant level of under-recording was found. In fact, in one of the case studies, over-recording seemed to have been the issue. This is in line with other published quantitative research that has shown that in fact mental state tags, particularly those in CCRs, tend to over identify mental state-related incidents (Kane et al., 2018). Studies that have, for example, used text mining algorithms can also over record because they often search on any mention of mental state/disorder or related terms regardless of the accuracy of their application, currency or, now, the likelihood that the new NPCC definition is being applied. It is also probable that the application of the new definition, where it occurred, has produced a more accurate reflection of actual demand rather the reliance on historic police systems flags, outdated local records and misattribution of mental state problems to an individual caller, for example. We know from other research that it appears that it is the application of the mental state flag, accurate or not, that triggers a differential response from police staff such as, for example, blue light responses, specialist team deployments and influences the on-street decision-making by officers (Kane et al., 2018). Improving the accuracy and application of this mental state flag in line with the NPCC & CoP definition is an area that could help manage demand more effectively than at present. Some forces already audit their mental state incidents in detail on a frequent basis and have been successful in weeding out some of the historic and inaccurate flags being applied. The new NPCC & CoP definition is also a step in the right direction if used across forces and rigorously backed up by clear, supportive force policies.

It is clear from our data that police staff, warranted and civilian, are dealing with a significant and potentially increasing number of vulnerable individuals; however, the data we have analysed in this study do not appear to support the contention that this demand is chiefly associated with mental state-related incidents. It seems likely, from the data we collected, that a significant number of the cases often being flagged as mental state-related incidents are in fact connected to other vulnerabilities rather than the individual's mental state. These most commonly include homelessness and drug and alcohol abuse. It is important if progress is to be made on understanding demand on policing created by all vulnerabilities, that the debate is broadened to include a wider range of national and local partners beyond just the Department of Health, National Health Service England and local health commissioners and service deliverers. In some areas, Crisis Concordat arrangements have helped develop more robust partnerships but in others partnership working around vulnerable individuals, including those experiencing mental state-related problems, appears weak. The Mental Health Crisis Care Concordat is a national agreement between services and agencies involved in the care and support of people in crisis. It sets out how organisations will work together better to make sure that people get the help they need when they are having a mental health crisis. Each area in England is required to action these arrangements locally (HM Government, 2014).

What we could not do in this project, and may be an avenue for future research, is to explore in detail whether there is any co-relation between the actual demand on policing and the performance and resources of other local public and third sector services, as well as mental health providers and commissioners. We did carry out some initial scoping of the health service aspects of this using open-source data and regression analysis, but the data were difficult to match at a sufficiently granular level and consequently the results were not clear and hence not reported here. More granular local data are needed to do justice to this area. Interestingly, it was clear from our interviews with police staff and other partner agency staff that demand was not perceived as high or as problematic where there were good relationships and trusted inter-agency ways of working, even though the data showed their demand was within the national profile and therefore broadly the same as other areas. Where these relationships and inter-agency working were not mature, demand was usually seen as high, even when falling firmly inside the national profile range. It is likely that many of the vulnerable individuals who are placing demands on police resources are in the remit of housing, social care, drug and alcohol services and related agencies, and it is partnership with them as well as health that will lead to reduced police demand and improved services for individuals.

It should also be borne in mind that given the general incidence of expressed mental state-related problems (around 1:6 individuals say they have experienced some mental state issues in the previous week (McManus et al., 2016) there will inevitably be contact between police and those who feel they are experiencing mental state-related problems. It is also important to recognise that the fact an individual is experiencing such problems does not of itself mean that there should be a differential police response unless their mental state has led to a particularly enhanced level of vulnerability or risk, for example.

5 LIMITATIONS

All data we present in this report are from police forces in England and Wales. We did find some data issues. Many of the forces found it a challenge to share complete data on mental state incidents, with consistency in some areas an issue. For example, when forces were asked about their data quality, some of the issues they reported included: 79% had mental state-related data recorded on a stand-alone system (as such the data were not integrated with the forces main systems and available to all who needed it), forces tended to think the data available to measure the time spent on mental state-related incidents were not accurate – 36% said they were very or fairly accurate, and 46% said the data were not very or not at all accurate, only two thirds of forces could provide data on the use of MHA Act 1983 section 136.

Data were gathered from a 24-hour snapshot and may not have been a typical profile for any given force. This is mitigated by the fact that when we looked at the three in-depth forces their 12-months data reflected the same or lower levels of mental state-related incidents.

We are aware that the use of the new definition was not consistent among forces, and this may also have had an effect on the numbers of incidents included by some forces, but should have helped the data from the forces using it to provide a more accurate reflection of demand. Our checks of mental state-related incidents in the case study forces did not suggest substantial over or under-recording of mental state-related incidents.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors are very grateful to many police staff, warranted and civilian, and their partner agencies who gave so much of their time to complete this work with them. Funding was granted by the Problem Solving and Demand Reduction Programme.

    CONFLICT OF INTEREST

    There were no conflicts of interest. The research team carried out the work independently and the findings reflect our views.

    DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

    Data used in this article are not available due to privacy or ethical reasons.

      The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.