Theory of Mind and Behavior
Amanda C. Brandone
Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania, USA
Search for more papers by this authorAmanda C. Brandone
Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania, USA
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
The capacity to understand and reason about the unobservable mental states (e.g., thoughts, desires, and beliefs) of oneself and others, known as theory of mind (ToM), is central to human social cognition. Multidisciplinary interest in ToM stems from its potentially unique human nature, the role it plays in our ability to engage in complex social interactions, and its impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders, such as autism. Through more than 30 years of research, we have learned a great deal about how and when children come to reason about others in terms of their mental states. This essay reviews foundational research on the development of ToM reasoning during childhood; outlines cutting-edge findings on the infant origins and neural correlates of ToM; and finally discusses key issues for future research, including reconciling infant competence with evidence of protracted conceptual development in early childhood, expanding our neuroscientific understanding of ToM and its development, and shedding light on the use and individual variability of ToM in everyday life. Pursuing these goals will address important theoretical questions and provide critical new insight into the origins, development, neural basis, and social and behavioral consequences of ToM.
References
- Apperly, I. A. (2011). Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of theory of mind. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970.
- Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
- Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
- Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., & Williams, S. C. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.
- Bowman, L. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Neuroscience contributions to childhood theory-of-mind development. In O. N. Saracho (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on research in theories of mind in early childhood education. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
- Brandone, A. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). You can't always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychological Science, 20, 85–91.
- Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.
- Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1063.
- Carpendale, J. I., & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Development, 67, 1686–1706.
- Carrington, S. J., & Bailey, A. J. (2009). Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 2313–35.
- Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 568–570.
-
Dunn, J., & Brophy, M. (2005). Communication, relationships and individual differences in children's understanding of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 50–69). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0003 Google Scholar
-
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–293). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511752902.011 Google Scholar
-
Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind & Language, 7, 120–144.
10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00201.x Google Scholar
- Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). 18-month-olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly. Infancy, 17(6), 672–691.
- Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in 'theory of mind'. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 529–533.
- Liszkowski, U. (2013). Using theory of mind. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 104–109.
- Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K. A., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., & Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others. Child Development, 45, 349–372.
-
Miller, S. A. (2012). Theory of mind: Beyond the preschool years. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
10.4324/9780203122730 Google Scholar
- Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622–646.
- Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7, F1–F9.
- Moore, C., & Frye, D. (1991). The acquisition and utility of theories of mind. In D. Frye & C. Moore (Eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 1–14). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.
- Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.
- Pillow, B. H. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116–129.
- Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
- Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–12.
- Sabbagh, M. A., Bowman, L. C., Evraire, L. E., & Ito, J. M. B. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. Child development, 80, 1147–62.
- Saxe, R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Development, 80, 1197–1209.
- Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587–592.
- Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906–912.
-
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
10.7551/mitpress/1811.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.
- Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541.
- Wellman, H. M., Lopez-Duran, S., LaBounty, J., & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 618–623.
- Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.
- Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128.
- Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.
- Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), Serial No. 27.
Further Reading
- Apperly, I. A. (2011). Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of theory of mind. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 110–118.
- Bowman, L. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Neuroscience contributions to childhood theory-of-mind development. In O. N. Saracho (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on research in theories of mind in early childhood education. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
- Doherty, M. J. (2009). Theory of mind: How children understand others' thoughts and feelings. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
-
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
10.7551/mitpress/1811.001.0001 Google Scholar
Citing Literature
Browse other articles of this reference work: