Theoretical Perspectives on the Development of Implicit and Explicit Prejudice
Frances E. Aboud
Search for more papers by this authorJennifer R. Steele
Search for more papers by this authorFrances E. Aboud
Search for more papers by this authorJennifer R. Steele
Search for more papers by this authorAdam Rutland
Search for more papers by this authorDrew Nesdale
Search for more papers by this authorChristia Spears Brown
Search for more papers by this authorSummary
This chapter defines prejudice and notes some of the many theories that have shaped the study of prejudice development. It summarizes the current empirical findings from research examining racial preferences and prejudice in early and late childhood, and attempts to explain these findings. Two theoretical mechanisms that are helpful for understanding the early high levels of explicit prejudice and implicit intergroup biases include brain maturation and experience with in-group and out-group faces. One interesting way to merge what people know of maturation, whether phrased as brain maturation or cognitive maturation, with social influences such as out-group contact, is to suggest that there are optimal windows of social influence. The concept of an optimal window has been used to explain language acquisition and preference. Exposure to one's mother tongue deletes synapses for foreign languages starting in infancy, so this perceptual narrowing for language may lead to stronger language preferences and prejudices as seen in some research.
References
- Aboud, F. E. (1981). Egocentrism, conformity, and agreeing to disagree. Developmental Psychology, 17, 791–799. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.6.791
- Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of ingroup favoritism and outgroup prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39, 48–60. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.48
- Aboud, F. E. (2005). The development of prejudice in childhood and adolescence. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 310–326). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
- Aboud, F. E. (2008). A social-cognitive developmental theory of prejudice. In S. M. Quintana & C. McKown (Eds.), Handbook of race, racism, and the developing child (pp. 55–71). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.
- Aboud, F. E., & Doyle, A. B. (1996a). Parental and peer influences on children's racial attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20, 371–383. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(96)00024-7
- Aboud, F. E., & Doyle, A. B. (1996b). Does talk of race foster prejudice or tolerance in children? Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 28, 161–170. doi: 10.1037/0008-400X.28.3.161
- Aboud, F. E., & Fenwick, V. (1999). Exploring and evaluating school-based interventions to reduce prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 767–785. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00146
- Aboud, F. E., Friedmann, J., & Smith, S. (2015). Direct and indirect friends in cross-ethnolinguistic peer relations. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 47(1), 68–79. doi:10.1037/a0037590
- Aboud, F. E., Mendelson, M. J., & Purdy, K. T. (2003). Cross-race peer relations and friendship quality. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 165–173. doi: 10.1080/01650250244000164
- Aboud, F. E., & Skerry, S. H. (1983). Self and ethnic concepts in relation to ethnic constancy. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 15, 14–26. doi: 10.1037/h0080675
- Aboud, F. E., Tredoux, C., Tropp, L. R., Brown, C. S., Niens, U., & Noor, N. M. (2012). Interventions to reduce prejudice and enhance inclusion and respect for ethnic differences in early childhood: a systematic review. Developmental Review, 32, 307–336. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2012.05.001
- Allport, G. W. (1954/1979). The nature of prejudice. New York, NY: Perseus Books.
- Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2013). Developmental origins of the other-race effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 173–178. doi: 10.1177/0963721412474459
- Augoustinos, M., & Rosewarne, D. L. (2001). Stereotype knowledge and prejudice in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 143–156. doi: 10.1348/026151001165912
- Banaji, M. R., Baron, A. S., Dunham, Y., & Olson, K. (2008). The development of intergroup social cognition: Early emergence, implicit nature, and sensitivity to group status. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 197–236). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
- Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x
- Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 39–89). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
- Brown, R. (1995). Prejudice: Its social psychology. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
- Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 201–234). New York: Academic Press.
- Davies, K., Tropp, L. R., Aron, A., Pettigrew, T. F., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Cross-group friendships and intergroup attitudes: a meta-analytic review. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 15, 332–351. doi:10.1177/1088868311411103
- Degner, J., & Dalege, J. (2013). The apple does not fall far from the tree, or does it? A meta-analysis of parent–child similarity in intergroup attitudes. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1270–1304. doi:10.1037/a0031436
- Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic activation of prejudice in children and adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 356–374. doi: 10.1037/a0017993
- Diesendruck, G., Goldfein-Elbaz, R., Rhodes, M., Gelman, S., & Neumark, N. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in children's beliefs about the objectivity of social categories. Child Development, 84, 1906–1917. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12108
- Doyle, A. B., & Aboud, F. E. (1995). A longitudinal study of white children's racial prejudice as a social-cognitive development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 209–228. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23090532
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Development, 77, 1268–1281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00933.x
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Children and social groups: A developmental analysis of implicit consistency among Hispanic-Americans. Self and Identity, 6, 238–255. doi: 10.1080/15298860601115344
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.006
- Dunham, Y., Newheiser, A., Hoosain, L., Merrill, A., & Olson, K. R. (2014). From a different vantage: Intergroup attitudes among children from low- and intermediate-status racial groups. Social Cognition, 32(1), 1–21.
- Gaither, S. E., Pauker, K., & Johnson, S. (2012). Biracial and monoracial infant own-race face perception: An eye-tracking study. Developmental Science, 15, 775–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01170.x
-
Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology ( 2nd ed., pp. 283–310). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511996481.016 Google Scholar
- Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (2010). Handbook of implicit social cognition. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Viswanath, K. (2008). Theory, research, and practice in health behavior and health education. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & K. Viswanath (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice ( 4th ed., pp. 23–40). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
- Gonzalez, A. M., Steele, J. R., & Baron, A. S. (in press). Reducing children's implicit racial bias through exposure to positive outgroup exemplars. Child Development.
- Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
- Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science, 14, 640–643. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1478.x
- Johnson, P. J., & Aboud, F. E. (2013). Modifying ethnic attitudes in young children: The impact of communicator race and message strength. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(3), 182–191. doi: 1.1177/0165025412466522
- Katz, P. A., & Zalk, S. R. (1978). Modification of children's racial attitudes. Developmental Psychology, 14, 447–461. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.14.5.447
- Kenworthy, J., Turner, R., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2005). Intergroup contact: When does it work, and why? In J. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 278–292). Oxford: Blackwell.
- Kinzler, K. D., & Dautel, J. B. (2012). Children's essentialist reasoning about language and race. Developmental Science, 15, 131–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01101.x
- Kinzler, K.D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E.S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children's social preferences. Social Cognition, 27, 623–626. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.4.623
- Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
- Lipman, C., Steele, J. R., & Williams, A. (2013, April). The effect of person construal on children's implicit racial attitudes. Paper presented at the fourth Implicit Bias, Philosophy and Psychology Conference in conjunction with the Leverhulme-funded Implicit Bias and Philosophy Project, Sheffield, UK.
- Maurer, D., & Werker, J. (2014). Perceptual narrowing during infancy: a comparison of language and faces. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 154–178. doi: 10.1002/dev.21177
- Monteiro, M., de França, D., & Rodrigues, R. (2009). The development of intergroup bias in childhood: How social norms can shape children's racial behaviors. International Journal of Psychology, 44, 29–39. doi: 10.1080/00207590802057910
- Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development and neural bases of face recognition. Infant and Child Development, 10, 3–18. doi: 10.1002/icd.239
- Nesdale, D. (2007). The development of ethnic prejudice in early childhood. In O. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on socialization and social development in early childhood education (pp. 213–240). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
- Newheiser, A., & Olson, K. R. (2012). White and Black American children's implicit intergroup bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 264–270. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.011
- Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 65–85. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65
- Piaget J., & Weil, A. M. (1951). The development in children of the idea of the homeland and of relations to other countries. International Social Science Journal, 3, 561–578.
- Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 1715–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x
- Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., & McGeorge, P. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children's implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76, 451–466. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00856.x
- Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
- Siegler, R. S., & Svetina, M. (2002). A microgenetic/cross-sectional study of matrix completion: Comparing short-term and long-term change. Child Development, 73, 793–809. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00439
- Siegler, R. S., & Svetina, M. (2006). What leads children to adopt new strategies? A microgenetic/cross-sectional study of class inclusion. Child Development, 77, 997–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00915.x
- Singh, R., Choo, W. M., & Poh, L. L. (1998). In-group bias and fair-mindedness as strategies of self-presentation in intergroup perception. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 147–162. doi: 10.1177/0146167298242004
- Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity, and social comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups (pp. 61–98). New York: Academic Press.
-
Tredoux, C. G., Noor, N. M., & de Paulo, L. (2009). Quantitative measures of respect and social inclusion in children: overview and recommendations. Effective Education, 1, 169–186. doi: 10.1080/19415530903522568
10.1080/19415530903522568 Google Scholar
- Verkuyten, M., & De Wolf, A. (2007). The development of in-group favoritism: Between social reality and group identity. Developmental Psychology, 43, 901–922. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.901
- Walker, P. M., Silvert, L., Hewstone, M., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Social contact and other-race face processing in the human brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 16–25. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm035
- Williams, A., & Steele, J. R. (2016). It's not you, it's me: The development of implicit racial attitudes in childhood . Manuscript under review.