Communication in Children's and Adolescents’ Social Groups
Patrick J. Leman
Search for more papers by this authorHarriet R. Tenenbaum
Search for more papers by this authorPatrick J. Leman
Search for more papers by this authorHarriet R. Tenenbaum
Search for more papers by this authorAdam Rutland
Search for more papers by this authorDrew Nesdale
Search for more papers by this authorChristia Spears Brown
Search for more papers by this authorSummary
This chapter discusses research into communication in childhood and adolescence in the context of work on group dynamics. It outlines the ways in which a better understanding of processes of communication can inform work in the area. Communication can be a useful diagnostic tool to understand how children and adolescents play out their group knowledge in everyday contexts. Research into the influence of gender and, to a lesser extent, ethnicity on children's and adolescents’ processes of communication highlights the sometimes profound effect of group memberships on the social dynamics of interaction. Communication in friendship groups may reinforce identification and ingroup norms. Friendship groups provide an opportunity for learning and rehearsing in-group norms of communication, while simultaneously limiting exposure to out-group norms. This has important implications for social developmental theory because communication within friendship groups therefore serves as a reinforcer of intergroup dynamics.
References
- Aboud, F. (1988). Children and prejudice. Oxford: Blackwell.
- Abrams, D., Palmer, S., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Van de Vyver, J. (2013). Evaluations of, and reasoning about, normative and deviant ingroup and outgroup members: Development of the Black Sheep Effect. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 258–270. doi: 10.1037/a0032461
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Ferrell, J., & Pelletier, J. (2008). Children's judgments of disloyal and immoral peer behaviour: Subjective group dynamics in minimal intergroup contexts. Child Development, 79, 444–446.
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Palmer, S. B., Pelletier, J., Ferrell, J., & Lee, S. (2014). The role of cognitive abilities in children's inferences about social atypicality and peer exclusion and inclusion in intergroup contexts. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32(3), 233–247. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12034
- Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.
- Archer, J. (1984). Gender roles as developmental pathways. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 245–256.
- Aydt, H., & Corsaro, W. A. (2003). Differences in children's construction of gender across culture: An interpretive approach. American Behavioral Scientist, 46, 1306–1325.
- Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children's understanding of emotion. Frontiers of Psychology, 4(670), 1–11.
- Banerjee, R., & Lintern, V. (2000). Boys will be boys: The effect of social evaluation concerns on gender-typing. Social Development, 9, 397–408.
- Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
- Berger, J., Hamit, F., Norman, R. Z., & Zelditch, M. (1977). Status characteristics and social interaction: An expectation states approach. New York, NY: Elsevier.
- Bergsieker, H., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected: Divergent goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 248–264.
- Bigler, R. S., Brown, C., & Markell, M. (2001). When groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 72, 1151–1162.
- Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L.S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children's social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166.
- Binder, J., Zagefka, H., Brown, R., Funke, F., Kessler, T., Mummendey, A., … Leyens, J.-P. (2009). Does contact reduce prejudice or does prejudice reduce contact? A longitudinal test of the Contact Hypothesis amongst majority and minority groups in three European countries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 843–856.
- Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Brown, R. J., & Douch, R. (2006). Changing children's intergroup attitudes towards refugees: Testing different models of extended contact. Child Development, 77, 1208–1219.
- Cartei, V., Cowles, W., Banerjee, R., & Reby, D. (2014). Control of voice gender in pre-pubertal children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32, 100–106. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12027
- Cervantes, A. A., & Callanan, M. (1998). Labels and explanations in mother-child emotion talk: Age and gender differentiation. Developmental Psychology, 34, 88–98.
-
Corsaro, W. A. (1990). The underlife of the nursery school. In G. Duveen & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Social representations and the development of knowledge (pp. 11–26). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511659874.002 Google Scholar
- Dalton, D., & Nesdale, D. (2011). Children's social groups and intergroup prejudice: Assessing the influence and inhibition of social group norms. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 895–909. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2010.02017.x
- Davis, S. C., Leman, P. J., & Barrett, M. (2007). Children's implicit and explicit ethnic group attitudes, ethnic group identification, and self-esteem. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 514–525.
- Di Donato, M., Martin, C. L., & England, H. (2014). Gendered interactions and their consequences: A dynamical perspective. In P. J. Leman & H. R. Tenenbaum (Eds.), Gender and development (pp. 133–154). London: Psychology Press.
- Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1983). The social development of the intellect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793–811. doi. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
- Duveen, G., & Lloyd, B. (1992). Gender identities and education. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
- Finkelstein, N. W., & Haskins, R. (1983). Kindergarten children prefer same color peers. Child Development, 54, 502–508.
- Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (2000). On the statistics of interdependence: Treating dyadic data with respect. In W. Ickes & S. Duck (Eds.), The social psychology of personal relationships (pp. 181–213). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
- Gummerum, M., Leman, P. J., & Hollins, T. (2014). How do children share information in groups? Developmental Psychology, 50, 2105–2114.
- Horwitz, S. R., Shutts, K., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Social class differences produce social group preferences. Developmental Science, 17(6), 991–1002.
- Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.
- Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
- James, D., & Drakich, J. (1993). Understanding gender differences in amount of talk: A critical review of research. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Gender and conversational interaction (pp. 281–312). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Jugert, P., Noack, P., & Rutland, A. (2013): Children's cross-ethnic friendships: Why are they less stable than same-ethnic friendships? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 649–662. doi:10.1080/17405629.2012.734136
- Katz, P. A., & Ksansnak, K. R. (1994). Developmental aspects of gender role flexibility and traditionality in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 272–282.
- Kruger, A. C. (1992). The effect of peer and adult–child transactive discussions on moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 191–211.
- Kyratzis, A. (2001). Children's gender indexing in language: From separate worlds hypothesis to considerations of culture, context and power. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 34, 1–13.
-
Kyratzis, A., & Tarim, S. (2010). Using directives to construct egalitarian or hierarchical social organization: Turkish middle class preschool girls’ socialisation about gender, affect, and context in peer group conversations. First Language, 30, 473–492.
10.1177/0142723710370547 Google Scholar
-
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511815355 Google Scholar
- Leaper, C. (1991). Influence and involvement in children's discourse: Age, gender, and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797–811. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01570.x
- Leaper, C. (1998). Decision-making processes between friends during a problem-solving task: Speaker and partner gender effects. Sex Roles, 39, 125–133.
- Leaper, C. (2000). Gender, affiliation, assertion, and the interactive context of parent–child play. Developmental Psychology, 36, 381–393. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.3.381
- Leaper, C. A., & Ayers, M. M. (2007). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in adults’ language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 11, 328–363. doi: 10.1177/1088868307302221
- Leaper, C., & Smith, T. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children's language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40, 993–1027.
- Leaper, C., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Shaffer, T. G. (1999). Gender effects on the communication strategies of African-American children from low-income, urban backgrounds. Child Development, 70, 1489–1503.
- Leman, P. J. (2015). How do groups work? Age differences in performance and the social outcomes of peer collaboration. Cognitive Science, 39(4), 809–820. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12172
- Leman, P. J., Ahmed, S., & Ozarow, L. (2005). Gender, gender relations, and the social dynamics of children's conversations. Developmental Psychology, 41, 64–74.
- Leman, P. J., & Björnberg, M. (2010). Gender, conversation, and development: A study of children's conceptions of punishment. Child Development, 81, 958–972. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01445.x
- Leman, P. J. and & Duveen, G. (1996). Developmental differences in children's understanding of epistemic authority. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 683–702.
- Leman, P. J., & Duveen, G. (1999). Representations of authority and children's moral reasoning. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 557–575.
- Leman, P. J., & Lam, V. L. (2008). The influence of race and gender on children's conversations and playmate choices. Child Development, 79, 1329–1343.
- Leman, P. J., Macedo, A. P., Bluschke, A., Hudson, L., Rawlings, C., & Wright, H. (2011). The influence of gender and ethnicity on children's peer collaborations. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 131–137. doi: 10.1348/026151010X526344
- Leman, P. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Practising gender: Children's relationships and the development of gendered behaviour and beliefs. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 153–157. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02032.x
- Lerner, J. (1978). Nature, nurture and dynamic interactionism. Human Development, 21, 1–20.
- Liben, L., & Bigler, R. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualising, measuring and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 6 (serial no. 269).
- Lourenco, O., & Machado, A. (1996). In defense of Piaget's theory: A reply to 10 common criticisms. Psychological Review, 103, 143–164.
- Maccoby, E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.
- Martin C. L., & Fabes R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences of young children's same-sex peer interactions, Developmental Psychology, 37, 431–446.
- Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Evans, S. M., & Wyman, H. (1999). Social cognition in the playground: Children's beliefs about playing with girls versus boys and their relations to sex-segregated play. Journal of Social and personal Relationships, 16, 751–771.
- Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramírez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). Are women really more talkative than men? Science, 317(5843), 82. doi: 10.1126/science.1139940
- Møller, S. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Danish majority children's reasoning about exclusion based on gender and ethnicity, Child Development, 82, 520–532. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01568.x.
- Pentland, A. (2010). To signal is human. American Scientist, 98, 204–211.
- Psaltis, C. & Duveen, G. (2006). Social relations and cognitive development: The influence of conversation type and representations of gender. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 407–430.
- Psaltis, C., Duveen, G., & Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (2009). The social and the psychological: Structure and context in human development. Human Development, 52, 291–312.
- Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 316–320.
-
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/oso/9780195059731.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Rutland, A. (1999). The development of national prejudice, in-group favouritism and self-stereotypes in British children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 55–70.
- Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Nigbur, D., Brown, R., Hossain, R., Landau, A., … Watters, C. (2012). Group identity and peer relations: A longitudinal study of group identity, perceived peer acceptance and friendships amongst ethnic minority English children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 283–302.
- Rutland, A., Killen, M., & Abrams, D. (2010). A new social-cognitive developmental perspective on prejudice: the interplay between morality and group identity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 279–291.
- Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 558–568.
- Serbin, L., Powlishta, K., & Gulko, J. (1993). The development of sex-typing in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(2) (serial no. 232).
- Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment. Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.
- Shutts, K., Roben, C. K. P., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Children's use of social categories in thinking about people and social relationships. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 35 –62. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.638686
- Siegler, R. (2006). How children develop. New York, NY: Worth.
- Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand: Men and women in conversation. New York, NY: Ballantine.
- Tenenbaum, H. R., Ford, S, & Alkhedairy, B. (2011) Telling stories: Gender differences in peers’ emotion talk and communication style. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(4), 707–721.
- Tenenbaum, H. R., & May, D. (2014). Gender in parent–child relationships. In P. J. Leman & H. R. Tenenbaum (Eds.), Gender and development (pp. 133–154). London: Psychology Press.
- Tenenbaum, H. R., Prior, J., Dowling, C. L., & Frost, R. E. (2010) Supporting parent–child conversations in a history museum. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 241–254. doi: 10.1348/000709909X470799
- Tenenbaum, H. R., Leman, P. J., & Aznar, A. (2015). Gender differences in language. In P. Brookes & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of Language Development. London: Sage.
- Tenenbaum, H. R. (2009). “You'd be good at that”: Gender patterns in parent–child talk about courses. Social Development, 18, 447–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00487.x.
- Trawalter, S., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2009). Predicting behavior during interracial interactions: A stress and coping approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 243–268.
- Valkenburg, P. M., Sumter, S., & Peter, J. (2011). Gender differences in online and offline self-disclosure in preadolescence and adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 253–269.
- Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 17, 89–100.