Toward a Contextualized Social Developmental Account of Children's Group-based Inclusion and Exclusion
The Developmental Model of Subjective Group Dynamics
Dominic Abrams
Search for more papers by this authorClaire Powell
Search for more papers by this authorSally B. Palmer
Search for more papers by this authorJulie Van de Vyver
Search for more papers by this authorDominic Abrams
Search for more papers by this authorClaire Powell
Search for more papers by this authorSally B. Palmer
Search for more papers by this authorJulie Van de Vyver
Search for more papers by this authorAdam Rutland
Search for more papers by this authorDrew Nesdale
Search for more papers by this authorChristia Spears Brown
Search for more papers by this authorSummary
This chapter focuses on how the linkage between intragroup and intergroup relations frames children's social inclusion and exclusion. It introduces subjective group dynamics theory to provide an account of this linkage, and explains the developmental subjective group dynamics (DSGD) model. This model considers children's expectations about how other group members may uphold group norms. The chapter also introduces propositions from the DSGD model. It seeks to explain how these SGD processes arise during social development and offers a series of propositions about children‘s judgments of group members in intergroup contexts, the underpinning social-cognitive abilities, and social experiences that contribute to group nous. The DSGD model holds that differential inclusion should relate to children's more advanced social perspective taking abilities. Theory of Social Mind” tests whether children can distinguish between their own negative evaluation of a thief from the evaluation by someone who had no knowledge of the character's wrongdoing.
References
- Abbott, N., & Cameron, L. (2014). What makes a young assertive bystander? The effect of intergroup contact, empathy, cultural openness and in-group bias on assertive bystander interventions. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 167–182. doi: 10.1111/josi.12053
- Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. Oxford: Blackwell.
- Abrams, D. (1988). Self-consciousness scales for adults and children: Reliability, validity and theoretical significance. European Journal of Personality, 2, 11–37. doi: 10.1002/per.2410020103
- Abrams, D. (1994). Social self-regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 473–484. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205004
- Abrams, D. (2004). The development of social identity: What develops? In M. Bennett, & F. Sani (Eds.), The development of the social self (pp. 291–312). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
- Abrams, D. (2011). Wherein lies children's intergroup bias? Egocentrism, social understanding and social projection. Child Development, 82, 1579–1593. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01617.x.
- Abrams, D., & Brown, R. J. (1989). Self-consciousness and social identity: Self-regulation as a group member. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52, 311–318.
-
Abrams, D., & Christian, J. N. (2007). A relational analysis of social exclusion. In D. Abrams, J. N. Christian, & D. Gordon (Eds.), Multidisciplinary handbook of social exclusion research (pp. 211–232). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
10.1002/9780470773178.ch12 Google Scholar
- Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (2004). Metatheory: Lessons from social identity research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 98–106. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0802_2
- Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2014). Social exclusion of children: Developmental origins of prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 1–11. doi: josi12043
- Abrams, D., Marques, J. M., Bown, N. J., & Henson, M. (2000). Pro-norm and anti-norm deviance within in-groups and out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 906–912. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.906
- Abrams, D., Palmer, S. B., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Van de Vyver, J. (2014). Evaluations of and reasoning about normative and deviant in-group and out-group members: Development of the black sheep effect. Developmental Psychology, 50, 258–270. doi: 10.1037/a0032461
- Abrams, D., Palmer, S. B., Van de Vyver, J., Hayes, D. P., Delaney, K., Guarella, S., & Purewal, K. (2016). Adolescents’ social judgments of doubly deviant peers: Implications of intergroup and intragroup dynamics for disloyal and overweight group members. Social Development. doi: 10.1111/sode.12187
- Abrams, D., Randsley de Moura, G. R., Hutchison, P., & Viki, G. T. (2005). When bad becomes good (and vice versa): Why social exclusion is not based on difference. In D. Abrams, M. A. Hogg, & J. M. Marques (Eds.), The social psychology of exclusion and inclusion (pp. 161–190). New York, NY: Psychology Press
- Abrams, D., & Rutland, A. (2011). Children's understanding of deviance and group dynamics: The development of subjective group dynamics. In J. Jetten & M. J. Hornsey (Eds.), Rebels in groups: Dissent, deviance, difference and defiance (pp. 135–157). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., & Cameron, L. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: Children's judgments of normative and deviant in-group and out-group individuals. Child Development, 74, 1840–1856. doi:10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00641.x
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Ferrell, J. (2007). Older but wilier: In-group accountability and the development of subjective group dynamics. Developmental Psychology, 43, 134–148. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.134
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Marques, J. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: When in-group bias gets specific. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 155–176. doi: 10.1348/026151003765264020
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Ferrell, J. M., & Pelletier, J. (2008). Children‘s judgments of disloyal and immoral peer behavior: Subjective group dynamics in minimal intergroup contexts. Child Development, 79, 444–461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01135.x
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Palmer, S. B., Pelletier, J., Ferrell, J., & Lee, S. (2014). The role of cognitive abilities in children's inferences about social atypicality and peer exclusion and inclusion in intergroup contexts. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32(3), 233–247. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12034
- Abrams, D. , Rutland, A., Palmer, S. B., & Purewal, K. (2014). Children's responses to social atypicality among group members – Advantages of a contextualized social-developmental account. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 257–261. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12053.
- Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Pelletier, J., & Ferrell, J. M. (2009). Children's group nous: Understanding and applying peer exclusion within and between groups. Child Development, 80, 224–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01256.x
- Abrams, D., Weick, M., Colbe, H., Thomas, D., & Franklin, K. M. (2011). On-line ostracism affects children differently from adolescents and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 110–123. doi:10.1348/026151010X494089
- M. Bennett, & F. Sani (Eds.). (2004). The development of the social self. Abingdon: The Psychology Press.
- Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1992). Cognitive mechanisms in children's gender stereotyping: Theoretical and educational implications of a cognitive-based intervention. Child Development, 63, 1351–1363. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01700.x
-
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 34, pp. 39–89). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
10.1016/S0065-2407(06)80004-2 Google Scholar
- Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull, & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 177–183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). Personality and social intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
- Castelli, L., Tomelleri, S., & Zogmaister, C. (2008). Implicit ingroup metafavoritism: Subtle preference for ingroup members displaying ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 807–818. doi:10.1177/0146167208315210
- Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2010). Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46, 437–445. doi:10.1037/a0017553
- Coull, G. J., Leekam, S. R., & Bennett, M. (2006). Simplifying second-order belief attribution: What facilitates children's performance on measures of conceptual understanding? Social Development, 15, 548–563. doi:10.1046/j.1467-9507.2006.00340.x
- Dunham, Y., & Emory, J. (2014). Of affect and ambiguity: The emergence of preference for arbitrary ingroups. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 81–98. doi: 0.1111/josi.12048.
- Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology. New York, NY: Free Press.
- Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental social psychology: From infancy to old age. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
- Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 1079–1083. doi:10.1038/nature07155
- Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). New York, NY: Academic Press.
- Heinze, J. E., & Horn, S. S. (2014). Heterosexual adolescents’ reasoning about social exclusion based on sexual orientation and gender conformity. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 63–82. doi: 10.1111/josi.12047
- Hitti, A., Mulvey, K. L., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2014). When is it okay to exclude a member of the ingroup? Children's and adolescents’ social reasoning. Social Development, 23, 451–469. doi: 10.1111/sode.12047
- Jones, S. E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Livingstone, A. G. (2011). Ganging up or sticking together? Group processes and children‘s responses to text-message bullying. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 71–96. doi 10.1348/000712610X502826
- Killen, M., Lee-Kim, J., McGlothlin, H., & Stangor, C. (2002). How children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion. Monographs of the society for research in child development (Vol. 67, No. 4). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
- Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., & Hitti, A. (2013). Social exclusion in childhood: A developmental intergroup perspective. Child Development, 84, 772–790. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12012
- Killen, M., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., Mulvey, K. L., & Hitti, A. (2013), Development of intra- and intergroup judgments in the context of moral and social-conventional norms. Child Development, 84, 1063–1080. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12011
-
Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (1994). Group socialization: Theory and research. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 305–336). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
10.1080/14792779543000093 Google Scholar
- Levy, S., & Dweck, C. (1999). The impact of children's static versus dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype formation. Child Development, 70, 1163–1180. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00085
- Liben, L. S. (2014). The individual ↔ context nexus in Developmental Intergroup Theory: Within and beyond the Ivory Tower. Research in Human Development, 11, 273–290, doi: 10.1080/15427609.2014.967048
- Marques, J. M., Abrams, D., Páez, D., & Hogg, M.A. (2001). Social categorization, social identification, and rejection of deviant group members. In M. A. Hogg and S. Tindale (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of social psychology (Vol. 3): Group Processes (pp. 400–424). Oxford: Blackwell.
- Marques, J. M., Abrams, D., Páez, D., & Martinez-Taboada, C. (1998). The role of categorization and in-group norms in judgments of groups and their members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 976–988. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.976
-
Marques, J. M., & Páez, D. (1994). The “black sheep effect”: Social categorization, rejection of in-group deviates and perception of group variability. European Review of Social Psychology, 5, 37–68.
10.1080/14792779543000011 Google Scholar
-
Marques, J. M., Páez, D., & Abrams, D. (1998). Social identity and intragroup differentiation as subjective social control. In J. F. Morales, D. Páez, J. C. Deschamps, and S. Worchel (Eds.), Current perspectives on social identity and social categorization (pp. 124–142). New York, NY: Sage.
10.4135/9781446279205.n9 Google Scholar
- Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J. (1988). The “black sheep effect”: Extremity of judgments towards ingroup members as a function of group identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 1–16. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420180102
- Mulvey, K. L., Hitti, A., Rutland, A., Killen, M., & Abrams, D. (2014). When do children dislike ingroup members? Resource allocation from individual and group perspectives. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 29–46. doi: 10.1111/josi.12045.
- Mulvey, K. L, Hitti, A., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2014). Context differences in children's ingroup preferences. Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1507–1519. doi: 10.1037/a0035593
-
Nesdale, D. (2008). Peer group rejection and children's intergroup prejudice. In S. R. Levy, & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 32–46). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/oso/9780195189742.003.0003 Google Scholar
- Nesdale, D., & Lawson, M. J. (2011). Social groups and children's intergroup attitudes: Can school norms moderate the effects of social group norms? Child Development, 82, 1594–1606. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01637.x
- Nesdale, D., Killen, M., & Duffy, A. (2013). Children's social cognition about proactive aggression. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 674–692. doi: org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.003
- Nesdale, D., Zimmer-Gembeck, M., & Roxburgh, N. (2014). Peer group rejection in childhood: Effects of rejection ambiguity, rejection sensitivity, and social acumen. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 12–28. doi: 10.1111/josi.12044
- Powell, C., Abrams, D., & Hopthrow, T. (2013). Brainstorming with children: A comparison of nominal and interacting groups . Paper presented at the INGRoup Annual Conference, Atlanta, GA.
- Prinstein, M. J., & Dodge, K. A. (2008). Current issues in peer influence research. In M. J. Prinstein, & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), Understanding peer influence in children and adolescents (pp. 3–13). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 1715–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x
- Rhodes, M. (2012). Naïve theories of social groups. Child Development, 83, 1900–1916. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01835.x
- Rigby, K. (1997). Attitudes and beliefs about bullying among Australian school children. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 18, 202–220.
- Rigby, K., & Bortolozzo, G. (2013). How schoolchildren's acceptance of self and others relate to their attitudes to victims of bullying. Social Psychology of Education, 16, 181–197. doi: 10.1007/s11218-013-9213-y
-
Rigby, K., & Johnson, B. (2006). Expressed readiness of Australian schoolchildren to act as bystanders in support of children who are being bullied. Educational Psychology, 26, 425–440. doi:10.1080/01443410500342047
10.1080/01443410500342047 Google Scholar
- Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon, & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 571–645). New York, NY: Wiley Publishers.
- Rutland, A. (2004). The development and self-regulation of intergroup attitudes in children. In M. Bennett and F. Sani (Eds.), The development of the social self (pp. 247–265). Abingdon: Psychology Press.
- Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., & McGeorge, P. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children's implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76, 451–466. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00856.x
-
Rutland, A., Abrams, D., & Cameron, L. (2006). Children's attitudes towards non-conformists: Intergroup relations and exclusion in middle childhood. International Journal of School Disaffection, 4, 45–52.
10.18546/IJSD.04.2.08 Google Scholar
- Schachter, S. (1951). Deviation, rejection, and communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 190–208.
- Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science, 14, 530–539. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01000.x
- Shaw, M. E. (1976). Group dynamics: The psychology of small group behavior. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
- Smetana, J. G. (2006). Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies and variations in children's moral and social judgments. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 119–154). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
- Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Williams, K. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641
- Williams, K., Forgas, J., & Von Hippel, W. (2005). The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection and bullying. New York, NY: Psychology Press