Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Long Road to Predicting Discrimination
Yarrow Dunham
Search for more papers by this authorYarrow Dunham
Search for more papers by this authorAdam Rutland
Search for more papers by this authorDrew Nesdale
Search for more papers by this authorChristia Spears Brown
Search for more papers by this authorSummary
The study of intergroup processes begins from many places, and a diversity of approaches in both theory and method is a hallmark and strength of the literature. This chapter focuses on the narrower topic of implicit forms of prejudice. It is natural to assume that prejudice causes discrimination, and that one reason to study prejudice is to understand how it gives rise to discrimination. Research on implicit intergroup attitudes emerged against a voluminous body of work on intergroup attitudes more generally. Most social and cognitive psychologists studying implicit social cognition had argued-or at least assumed-that phenomena such as implicit prejudice emerged gradually through a “slow learning” internalization of statistical regularities in the environment. Early studies on the development of implicit intergroup bias focused on majority children in the United States, Japan, and the United Kingdom, and employed the Implicit Association Test.
References
- Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. New York: Blackwell.
- Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of in-group favoritism and out-group prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 48–60. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.48
- Aboud, F., Mendelson, M., & Purdy, K. (2003). Cross-race peer relations and friendship quality. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 165–173.
- Apfelbaum, E. P., Norton, M. I., & Sommers, S. R. (2012). Racial color blindness emergence, practice, and implications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(3), 205–209.
- Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Learning (not) to talk about race: When older children underperform in social categorization. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1513–1518. doi:10.1037/a0012835
- Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or “would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the implicit association test?” Psychological Inquiry, 15(4), 257–278.
- Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(1), 53–58.
- Barrett, M. (2007). Children's knowledge, beliefs and feelings about nations and national groups. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 68(3), 530–543.
- Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental Intergroup Theory explaining and reducing children's social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166.
- Bigler, R. S., Spears Brown, C., & Markell, M. (2001). When groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 72(4), 1151–1162.
- Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Klick, J., Mellers, B., Mitchell, G., & Tetlock, P. E. (2009). Strong claims and weak evidence: reassessing the predictive validity of the IAT. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(3), 567.
- Brand, E. S., Ruiz, R. A., & Padilla, A. M. (1974). Ethnic identification and preference: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 81(11), 860.
- Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 307.
- Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444.
- Cameron, J. A., Alvarez, J. M., Ruble, D. N., & Fuligni, A. J. (2001). Children's lay theories about ingroups and outgroups: Reconceptualizing research on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(2), 118–128.
-
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367638.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 469.
- Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic prejudice in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 356.
- Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 62.
- Dunham, Y. (2011). An angry = Outgroup effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 668–671. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.01.003
- Dunham, Y. (2015). Rethinking the development of race bias: The role of category salience . Unpublished manuscript.
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Development, 77(5), 1268–1281.
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Children and social groups: A developmental analysis of implicit consistency in Hispanic Americans. Self and Identity, 6(2–3), 238–255. doi:10.1080/15298860601115344
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.006
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2015). The development of implicit gender attitudes. Developmental Science, 18, 469–483. doi: 10.1111/desc.12321
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82(3), 793–811. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
- Dunham, Y., Chen, E. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental invariance and early enculturation. Psychological Science, 24(6), 860–868. doi:10.1177/0956797612463081
-
Dunham, Y., & Degner, J. (2013). From categories to exemplars (and back again). In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 275–280). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0050 Google Scholar
- Dunham, Y., & Emory, J. (2014). Of affect and ambiguity: The emergence of preference for arbitrary ingroups. Journal of Social Issues, 70(1), 81–98.
- Dunham, Y., Newheiser, A. K., Hoosain, L., Merrill, A., & Olson, K. R. (2014). From a different vantage: Intergroup attitudes among children from low-and intermediate-status racial groups. Social Cognition, 32(1), 1–21.
-
Dunham, Y., & Olson, K. R. (2008). The importance of origins: Why cognitive development is central to a mature understanding of social cognition. The Open Psychology Journal, 1, 59–65.
10.2174/1874350100801010059 Google Scholar
- Dunham, Y., Srinivasan, M., Dotsch, R., & Barner, D. (2014). Religion insulates ingroup evaluations: the development of intergroup attitudes in India. Developmental Science, 17(2), 311–319.
- Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 297–327.
- Finkelstein, N. W., & Haskins, R (1983). Kindergarten children prefer same-color peers. Child Development, 54, 502–508.
- Fishbein, H. D. (1996). Peer prejudice and discrimination: Evolutionary, cultural, and developmental dynamics. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
- B. Gawronski, & B. K. Payne (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford Press.
- Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 509–517.
- Girouard, N., Stack, D. M., & O'Neill-Gilbert, M. (2011). Ethnic differences during social interactions of preschoolers in same-ethnic and cross-ethnic dyads. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(2), 185–202.
- Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.
- Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464.
- Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Heiphetz, L., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes in children and adults: Tests in the domain of religion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 864–879.
- Hermans, D., de Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1994). The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. Cognition & Emotion, 8(6), 515–533.
- Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 575–604.
-
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1998). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child's construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
10.7551/mitpress/5734.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Horwitz, S. R., Shutts, K., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Social class differences produce social group preferences. Developmental Science, 17(6), 991–1002.
- Hraba, J., & Grant, G. (1970). Black is beautiful: A reexamination of racial preference and identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(3), 398.
- Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E. P., Johnson, D. J., Stevenson, H. C., & Spicer, P. (2006). Parents’ ethnic-racial socialization practices: A review of research and directions for future study. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 747–770.
- Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 16–32.
- Katz, P. A. (1983). Developmental foundations of gender and racial attitudes. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.), The child's construction of social inequality (pp. 41–78). New York: Academic Press.
- Killen, M. (2007). Children's social and moral reasoning about exclusion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(1), 32–36.
- Linville, P. W. (1985). Self-complexity and affective extremity: Don't put all of your eggs in one cognitive basket. Social Cognition, 3(1), 94–120.
- McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, racism, and discrimination (pp. 91–126). New York, NY: Academic Press.
- Moody, J. (2001). Race, school integration, and friendship segregation in America, 1. American Journal of Sociology, 107(3), 679–716.
- Nesdale, D., & Flesser, D. (2001). Social identity and the development of children's group attitudes. Child Development, 72(2), 506–517.
- Nesdale, D., Griffiths, J. A., Durkin, K., & Maass, A. (2007). Effects of group membership, intergroup competition and out-group ethnicity on children's ratings of in-group and out-group similarity and positivity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 359–373.
- Nesdale, D., Maass, A., Griffiths, J., & Durkin, K. (2003). Effects of in-group and out-group ethnicity on children's attitudes towards members of the in-group and out-group. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 177–192.
- Newheiser, A.-K., Dunham, Y., Merrill, A., Hoosain, L., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Preference for high status predicts implicit outgroup bias among children from low-status groups. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1081–1090. doi:10.1037/a0035054
- Newheiser, A.-K., & Olson, K. R. (2012). White and Black American children's implicit intergroup bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 264–270.
- Nosek, B. A. (2007). Implicit–explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 65–69.
- Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101–115.
- Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., … Banaji, M. R. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18(1), 36–88.
- Oakes, P. J. (1987). The salience of social categories. In J. C. Turner (Ed.), Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory (pp. 117–141). Oxford: Blackwell.
- Olson, K. R., & Dunham, Y. (2010). The development of implicit social cognition. In B. Gawronski, & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 241–254). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.
- Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children's attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77(4), 847–860.
- Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–293.
-
Phinney, J. S. (1989). Stages of ethnic identity development in minority group adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 9(1–2), 34–49.
10.1177/0272431689091004 Google Scholar
- Porter, J. (1971). Black child, white child: The development of racial attitudes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Quintana, S. M. (1998). Children's developmental understanding of ethnicity and race. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 7, 27–45.
- Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 1715–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x
-
Ramsey, P. G., & Myers, L. C. (1990). Salience of race in young children's cognitive, affective, and behavioral responses to social environments. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11(1), 49–67.
10.1016/0193-3973(90)90031-E Google Scholar
- Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(2), 88–106.
- Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45(9), 1043–1056.
- Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the implicit association test: Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 139–165.
- Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., & McGeorge, P. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children's implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76(2), 451–466.
- Shrum, W., Cheek, N. H., Jr., & Hunter, S. M. (1988). Friendship in school: Gender and racial homophily. Sociology of Education, 61(4), 227–239.
- Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108–131.
- Stevenson, H. W., & Stevenson, N. G. (1960). Social inter-action in an interracial nursery school. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 61, 37–75.
- Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223(5), 96–102.
- Xu, F., & Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105(13), 5012–5015.
- Yee, M., & Brown, R. (1994). The development of gender differentiation in young children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 183–196.