Effects of cigarette smoking on time estimation
M. C. Carrasco
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorR. Redolat
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
V. M. Simón
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Blasco Ibañez 21. Universidad de Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain.Search for more papers by this authorM. C. Carrasco
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorR. Redolat
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
V. M. Simón
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Area de Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Blasco Ibañez 21. Universidad de Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain.Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Time estimation was measured in 33 heavy smokers, 34 non-smokers and 23 ex-smokers over two experimental sessions. All smokers abstained from smoking for 30 min prior to each session. Half of the smokers smoked a cigarette prior to time estimation measurement in the first session, whereas the remaining smokers smoked in the second one. Non-smokers and ex-smokers did not smoke. Results combined across the two sessions indicated no differences between smoking versus non-smoking conditions. However, a specific pattern of responses appeared in each experimental session. Separate analyses of results from each session revealed that, in the first session, smokers that smoked overestimated time in comparison with abstinent smokers, non-smokers and ex-smokers. In contrast, results of the second session showed no significant effects of smoking or abstinence. These results indicate that the effects of nicotine on time perception can be influenced by the repetition of the task and by the experimental situation. Data suggest that there may exist an interaction between the order of treatment (smoking versus non-smoking) and time estimation. Copyright © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
References
- Aguè, C. (1974). Cardiovascular variables, skin conductance and time estimation: Changes after the administration of small doses of nicotine. Psycho-pharmacologia (Berl.), 37, 109–125.
- American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
- Balfour, D. J. K. and Fagerström, K. O. (1996). Pharmacology of nicotine and its therapeutic use in smoking cessation and neurodegenerative disorders. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 72, 51–81.
- Benowitz, N. L., Porchet, H. and Jacob, P., III (1990). Psychopharmacokinetics, metabolism, and pharmaco-dynamics of nicotine. In Nicotine Psychopharmacology, S. Wonnacott, M. A. H. Russell and I. P. Stolerman (Eds), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 112–157.
- Bizot, J. C. (1997). Effects of psychoactive drugs on temporal discrimination in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 8, 293–308.
- Block, R. A. and Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 184–197.
- Boltz, M. (1991). Time estimation and attentional perspective. Perception and Psychophysics, 49, 422–433.
- Brown, S. W. (1985). Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 115–124.
- Carrasco, M. C., Redolat, R. and Simön, V. M. (in press). Time estimation in minimally abstinent smokers. Human Psychopharmacology.
- Clausen, J. (1950). An evaluation of experimental methods of time judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 756–761.
- Conners, C. K., Levin, E. D., Sparrow, E., Hinton, S. C., Erhardt, D., Meck, W. H., Rose, J. E. and March, J., (1996). Nicotine and attention in adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Psycho-pharmacology Bulletin, 32, 67–73.
- Fant, R. V., Shuh, K. J. and Stitzer, M. L. (1995). Response to smoking as a function of prior smoking amounts. Psychopharmacology, 119, 385–390.
- Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 1–36.
- Fuxe, K., Andersson, K., Härfstrand, A., Eneroth, P., Perez, M. and Agnati, L. F. (1990). Effects of nicotine on synaptic transmission in the brain. In Nicotine Psychopharmacology, S. Wonnacott, M. A. H. Russell and I. P. Stolerman (Eds), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 194–225.
- Hasenfratz, M. and Bättig, K. (1993). Effects of smoking on cognitive performance and psychophysiological parameters as a function of smoking state. Human Psychopharmacology, 8, 335–344.
- Hasenfratz, M., Nil, R. and Bättig, K. (1990). Development of central and peripheral smoking effects over time. Psychopharmacology, 101, 359–365.
- Hatsukami, D. K., Pickens, R. W., Svikis, D. S. and Hughes, J. R. (1988). Smoking topography and nicotine blood levels. Addictive Behaviors, 13, 91–95.
-
Heatherton, T. F.,
Kozlowski, L. T.,
Frecker, R. C. and
Fagerström, K. O.
(1991).
The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire.
British Journal of Addiction,
86,
1110–1127.
10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x Google Scholar
- Herning, R. I., Jones, R. T., Benowitz, N. L. and Mines, A. H. (1983). How a cigarette is smoked determines blood nicotine levels. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 33, 84–90.
- Hicks, R. E. and Miller, G. W. (1976). Transfer of time judgment as a function of feedback. American Journal of Psychology, 89, 303–310.
- Hicks, R. E., Miller, G. W. and Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processes. American Journal of Psychology, 89, 719–730.
- Hopkins, R., Wood, L. E. and Sinclair, N. M. (1984). Evaluation of methods to estimate cigarette smoke update. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 36, 788–795.
- Ivry, R. B. (1993). Cerebellar involvement in the explicit representation of temporal information. In Temporal Information Processing in the Nervous System: Special Reference to Dyslexia and Aphasia, P. Tallal, A. Galaburda, R. R. Llinas and C. Von Euler,(Eds), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp. 214–230.
- Kadoya, C., Domino, E. F. and Matsuoka, S., (1994). Relationship of electroencephalographic and cardiovascular changes to plasma nicotine levels in tobacco smokers. Pharmacodynamics and Drug Action, 55, 370–377.
- Leigh, G. and Tong, J. E. (1976). Effects of ethanol and tobacco on time judgment. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 899–903.
- Levin, E. D., Conners, C. K., Sparrow, E., Hinton, S. C., Erhardt, D., Meck, W. H., Rose, J. E. and March, J., (1996). Nicotine effects on adults with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology, 123, 55–63.
- Lucchesi, B. R., Schuster, C. R. and Emley, G. S. (1967). The role of nicotine as a determinant of cigarette smoking frequency in man with observations of certain cardiovascular effects associated with the tobacco alkaloid. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 8, 789–796.
- Lyvers, M. and Miyata, Y. (1993). Effects of cigarette smoking on electrodermal orienting reflexes to stimulus change and stimulus significance. Psychophysiology, 30, 231–236.
- Maquet, P., Lejeune, H., Pouthas, V., Bonnet, M., Casini, L., Macar, F., Timsit-Berthier, M., Vidal, F., Ferrara, A., Degueldre, C., Quaglia, L., Delfiore, G., Luxen, A., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J. C. and Comar, D. (1996). Brain activation induced by estimation of duration: A PET study. Neuroimage, 3, 119–126.
- Meck, W. H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 227–242.
- Meck, W. H. and Church, R. M. (1987). Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 457–464.
- Meck, W. H., Church, R. M., Wenk, G. L. and Olton, D. S. (1987). Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 3505–3511.
- Nichelli, P., Clark, K., Hollnagel, C. and Grafman, J., (1995). Duration processing after frontal lobe lesions. In Structure and Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex, J. Grafman, K. J. Holyoak and F. Boller,(Eds), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp. 183–190.
- Olton, D. S. (1989). Frontal cortex, timing and memory. Neuropsychologia, 27, 121–130.
-
Pastor, M. A. and
Artieda, J.
(1996).
Involvement of the basal ganglia in timing perceptual and motor tasks.
In Time, Internal Clocks and Movement,
M. A. Pastor and
J. Artieda, (Eds),
Elsevier, Amsterdam,
pp. 235–255.
10.1016/S0166-4115(96)80062-8 Google Scholar
- Petrie, R. X. and Deary, I. J. (1989). Smoking and human information processing. Psychopharmacology, 99, 393–396.
- Predebon, J. (1996). The effects of active and passive processing of interval events on prospective and retrospective time estimates. Acta Psychologica, 94, 41–58.
- Pritchard, W. S., Robinson, J. H. and Guy, T. D. (1992). Enhancement of continuous performance task reaction time by smoking in non-deprived smokers. Psychopharmacology, 108, 437–442.
- Provost, S. C. and Woodward, R. (1991). Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop test. Psychopharmacology, 104, 536–540.
- Rammsayer, T. H. (1993). On dopaminergic modulation of temporal information processing. Biological Psychology, 36, 209–222.
- Redolat, R., Carrasco, M. C. and Simón, V. M. (1994). Efectos cognitivos de la nicotina y el tabaco en sujetos humanos. Psicothema, 6, 5–20.
- Rusted, J., Graupner, L., O'Connell, N. and Nicholls, C., (1994). Does nicotine improve cognitive function? Psychopharmacology, 115, 547–549.
- Sherwood, N. (1993). Effects of nicotine on human psychomotor performance. Human Psychopharmacology, 8, 155–184.
- Sherwood, N. (1995). Effects of cigarette smoking on performance in a simulated driving task. Neuropsychobiology, 32, 161–165.
- Simón, V. M., Redolat, R., Parra, A., Carrasco, C. and Somoza, E. (1991). A computerized system for measuring time perception in human subjects. International Journal of Bio-Medical Computing, 29, 95–118.
- Somoza, E. and Parra, A. (1995). A computerized procedure for developing and administering time perception protocols: Application to a sample of adults and adolescents. International Journal of Bio-Medical Computing, 40, 41–49.
-
Spielberger, C. D.,
Gorsuch, R. L. and
Lushene, R. E.
(1970).
Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.
10.1037/013734 Google Scholar
- Stolerman, I. P. (1991). Behavioural pharmacology of nicotine: Multiple mechanisms. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 533–536.
- Stolerman, I. P., Mirza, N. R. and Shoaib, M., (1995). Nicotine psychopharmacology: Addiction, cognition and neuroadaptation. Medical Research Reviews, 15, 47–72.
- Tong, J. E., Booker, J. L. and Knott, V. J. (1978). Effects of tobacco, time on task, and stimulus speed on judgments of velocity and time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 175–178.
- Warburton, D. M. (1990). Psychopharmacology aspects of nicotine. In Nicotine Psychopharmacology, S. Wonnacott, M. A. H. Russell and I. P. Stolerman (Eds), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 77–111.
- Warburton, D. M. and Arnall, C. (1994). Improvements in performance without nicotine withdrawal. Psychopharmacology, 115, 539–542.
- Woodson, P. P. and Griffiths, R. R. (1992). Control of cigarette smoking topography: Smoke filtration and draw resistance. Behavioral Pharmacology, 3, 99–111.
- Zakay, D., Nitzan, D. and Glicksohn, J. (1983). The influence of task difficulty and external tempo on subjective time estimation. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 451–456.