RELATIONAL LEARNING IN CHILDREN WITH DEAFNESS AND COCHLEAR IMPLANTS
Ana Claudia Almeida-Verdu
UNIVERSIDADE ESTADUAL PAULISTA, BRAZIL
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Deisy G. De Souza
UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL DE SÃO CARLOS, BRAZIL
Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal de Sao Carlos, Caixa Postal 676; 13565-905 Sao Carlos, SP, Brazil (e-mail: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorAna Claudia Almeida-Verdu
UNIVERSIDADE ESTADUAL PAULISTA, BRAZIL
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Deisy G. De Souza
UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL DE SÃO CARLOS, BRAZIL
Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal de Sao Carlos, Caixa Postal 676; 13565-905 Sao Carlos, SP, Brazil (e-mail: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This four-experiment series sought to evaluate the potential of children with neurosensory deafness and cochlear implants to exhibit auditory-visual and visual-visual stimulus equivalence relations within a matching-to-sample format. Twelve children who became deaf prior to acquiring language (prelingual) and four who became deaf afterwards (postlingual) were studied. All children learned auditory-visual conditional discriminations and nearly all showed emergent equivalence relations. Naming tests, conducted with a subset of the children, showed no consistent relationship to the equivalence-test outcomes. This study makes several contributions to the literature on stimulus equivalence. First, it demonstrates that both pre- and postlingually deaf children can acquire auditory-visual equivalence relations after cochlear implantation, thus demonstrating symbolic functioning. Second, it directs attention to a population that may be especially interesting for researchers seeking to analyze the relationship between speaker and listener repertoires. Third, it demonstrates the feasibility of conducting experimental studies of stimulus control processes within the limitations of a hospital, which these children must visit routinely for the maintenance of their cochlear implants.
REFERENCES
- Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17–29.
- da Costa, A. R., Wilkinson, K. M., de Souza, D. G., & McLlvane, W. J. (2001). Emergent word-object mapping by children: Further studies using the blank comparison technique. The Psychological Record, 51, 343–355.
- da Silva, W. R., de Souza, D. G., de Rose, J. C., Lopes Jr., J., Bevilacqua, M. C., & McLlvane, W. J. (2006). Relational learning in deaf children with cochlear implants. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 24, 1–8.
- de Rose, J. C., de Souza, D. G., & Hanna, E. S. (1996). Teaching reading and spelling: Exclusion and stimulus equivalence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 451–469.
- Dixon, L. S. (1977). The nature of control by spoken words over visual stimulus selection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 433–442.
- Dube, W. V. (1991). Computer software for stimulus control research with Macintosh computers. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 9, 28–39.
- Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test — Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
- Ferrari, C., de Rose, J. C., & McLlvane, W. J. (1993). Exclusion vs. selection training of auditory-visual conditional relations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 49–63.
- Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125–155.
- Guess, D. (1969). A functional analysis of receptive language and productive speech: Acquisition of the plural morpheme. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 55–64.
- Holth, P., & Arntzen E. (1998). Stimulus familiarity and the delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence or consistent nonequivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 81–110.
- Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: oxford.
- Kileny, P. R., Zwolan, T. A., & Ashbaugh, C. (2001). The influence of age at implantation on performance with a cochlear implant in children. Otology and Neurotology, 22, 42–46.
- Lee, V. L. (1981). Prepositional phrases spoken and heard. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 227–242.
- Markman, E. M. (1987). How children constrain the possible meaning of words. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- McLlvane, W. J., & Dube, W. V. (1992). Stimulus control shaping and stimulus control topographies. The Behavior Analyst, 15, 89–94.
- McLlvane, W. J., & Dube, W. V. (2003). Stimulus control topography coherence theory: Foundations and extensions. The Behavior Analyst, 26, 195–213.
- McLlvane, W. J., Kledaras, J. B., Stoddard, L. T., & Dube, W. V. (1990). Delayed sample presentation in MTS: Some possible advantages for teaching individuals with developmental limitations. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 8, 31–33.
- McLlvane, W. J., Munson, L. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988). Some observations on control by spoken words in childrens' conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 472–495.
- McLlvane, W. J., & Stoddard, L. T. (1981). Acquisition of matching-to-sample performances in severe retardation: Learning by exclusion. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 25, 33–48.
- O'Donoghue, G. M., Nikolopoulos, T. P., & Archbold, S. M. (2000). Determinants of speech perception in children after cochlear implantation. Lancet, 356, 466–468.
- Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1990). Conditional discrimination in mentally-retarded adults—the development of generalized skills. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 239–250.
- Saunders, R. R., Wachter, J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95–115.
- Sidman, M. (1977). Teaching some basic prerequisites for reading. In P. Mittler (Ed.), Research to practice in mental retardation, Vol. II (pp. 353–360). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
- Sidman, M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1967). The effectiveness of fading in programming a simultaneous form discrimination for retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 3–15.
- Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discriminations vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.
-
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
10.1037/11256-000 Google Scholar
- Spradlin, J. E., & Saunders, R. R. (1986). The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification vs. comparison classification. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 41–58.
- Stoddard, L. T., de Rose, J. C., & McLlvane, W. J. (1986). Observacoes curiosas acerca do desempenho deficiente apos a ocorrencia de erros [Curious observations on defective performance after errors]. Psicologia, 12(1), 1–18.
- Stoddard, L. T., Stoddard, M. (1967). The effects of errors on children's performance on a circle-ellipse discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 261–270.
- Svirsky, M. A., Robbins, A. M., Kirk, K. I., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2000). Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychological Science, 11, 153–158.
- Svirsky, M. A., Teoha, S., & Neuburger, H. (2004). Development of language and speech perception in congenitally, profoundly deaf children as a function of age at cochlear implantation. Audiology & NeuroOtology, 9, 224–233.
- Wilkinson, K. M., Dube, W. V., & McLlvane, W. J. (1996). A crossdisciplinary perspective on studies of rapid word mapping in psycholinguistics and behavior analysis. Developmental Review, 16, 125–148.