Using ERPs to investigate valence processing in the affect misattribution procedure
Corresponding Author
Curtis D. Von Gunten
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Address correspondence to: Curtis Von Gunten, 119 Psychology Bldg., University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorBruce D. Bartholow
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Search for more papers by this authorLaura D. Scherer
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Curtis D. Von Gunten
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Address correspondence to: Curtis Von Gunten, 119 Psychology Bldg., University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorBruce D. Bartholow
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Search for more papers by this authorLaura D. Scherer
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
Search for more papers by this authorPreparation of this article was supported in part by grant R01 AA020970 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
Abstract
The construct validity of the affect misattribution procedure (AMP) has been challenged by theories proposing that the task does not actually measure affect misattribution. The current study tested the validity of the AMP as a measure of affect misattribution by examining three components of the ERP known to be associated with the allocation of motivated attention. Results revealed that ERP amplitudes varied in response to affectively ambiguous targets as a function of the valence of preceding primes. Furthermore, differences in ERP responses to the targets were largely similar to differences in ERPs elicited by the primes. The existence of valence differentiation in both the prime-locked and the target-locked ERPs, along with the similarity in this differentiation, provides evidence that the affective content of the primes is psychologically registered, and that this content influences the processing of the subsequent, evaluatively ambiguous targets, both of which are required if the priming effects found in the AMP are the result of affect misattribution. However, the behavioral priming effect was uncorrelated with ERP amplitudes, leaving some question as to the locus of this effect in the information-processing system. Findings are discussed in light of the strengths and weaknesses of using ERPs to understand the priming effects in the AMP.
References
- Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
- Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Reporting intentional rating of the primes predicts priming effects in the affective misattribution procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1194–1208. doi: 10.1177/0146167212446835
- Bartholow, B. D., Riordan, M. A., Saults, J. S., & Lust, S. A. (2009). Psychophysiological evidence of response conflict and strategic control of responses in affective priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 655–666. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.015
- Begleiter, H., & Platz, A. (1969). Cortical evoked potentials to semantic stimuli. Psychophysiology, 6, 91–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1969.tb02887.x
- Blaison, C., Imhoff, R., Hühnel, I., Hess, U., & Banse, R. (2012). The affect misattribution procedure: Hot or not? Emotion, 12, 403–412. doi: 10.1037/a0026907
- Briggs, K. E., & Martin, F. H. (2008). Target processing is facilitated by motivationally relevant cues. Biological Psychology, 78, 29–42. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.12.007
- Briggs, K. E., & Martin, F. H. (2009). Affective picture processing and motivational relevance: Arousal and valence effects on ERPs in an oddball task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.01.009
- Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L. J., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Attitudes to the right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late positive event-related brain potentials. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1205–1219. doi: 10.1177/01461672962212002
- Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 330–350. doi: 10.1177/1088868312440047
- Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2006). Repetitive picture processing: Autonomic and cortical correlates. Brain Research, 1068, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.009
- Crites, S. L., Jr., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Electrocortical differentiation of evaluative and nonevaluative categorizations. Psychological Science, 7, 318–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00381.x
- Delplanque, S., Silvert, L., Hot, P., & Sequeira, H. (2005). Event-related P3a and P3b in response to unpredictable emotional stimuli. Biological Psychology, 68, 107–120. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.04.006
- Delplanque, S., Silvert, L., Hot, P., Rigoulot, S., & Sequeira, H. (2006). Arousal and valence effects on event-related P3a and P3b during emotional categorization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.06.006
- Edwards, L. J., Muller, K. E., Wolfinger, R. D., Qaqish, B. F., & Schabenberger, O. (2008). An R2 statistic for fixed effects in the linear mixed model. Statistics in Medicine, 27, 6137–6157. doi: 10.1002/sim.3429
- Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
- Ferrari, V., Codispoti, M., Cardinale, R., & Bradley, M. M. (2008). Directed and motivated attention during processing of natural scenes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1753–1761. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20121
- Flaisch, T., Junghöfer, M., Bradley, M. M., Schupp, H. T., & Lang, P. J. (2008). Rapid picture processing: Affective primes and targets. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00600.x
- Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Schupp, H. T. (2008). Affective prime and target picture processing: An ERP analysis of early and late interference effects. Brain Topography, 20(4), 183–191. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0045-6
- Freunberger, R., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., & Höller, Y. (2007). Visual P2 component is related to theta phase-locking. Neuroscience Letters, 426, 181–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.08.062
- Gable, P. A., Adams, D. L., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). Transient tasks and enduring emotions: The impacts of affective content, task relevance, and picture duration on the sustained late positive potential. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 45–54. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0313-8
- Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Late positive potential to appetitive stimuli and local attentional bias. Emotion, 10(3), 441. doi: 10.1037/a0018425
-
Gawronski, B., &
De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In A. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology, 2 (pp. 283–310). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511996481.016
10.1017/cbo9780511996481.016 Google Scholar
- Gawronski, B., & Ye, Y. (2014a). What drives priming effects in the affect misattribution procedure? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 3–15. doi: 10.1177/0146167213502548
- Gawronski, B., & Ye, Y. (2014b). Prevention of intention invention in the affect misattribution procedure. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 101–108. doi: 10.1177/1948550614543029
- Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
- Hajcak, G., Dunning, J. P., & Foti, D. (2009). Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: Time-course of the late positive potential. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 505–510. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.028
- Hajcak, G., & Olvet, D. M. (2008). The persistence of attention to emotion: Brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion, 8(2), 250. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.250
- Hashimoto, Y., Minami, T., & Nakauchi, S. (2012). Electrophysiological differences in the processing of affect misattribution. PLOS ONE, 7(11), e49132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049132
- Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45, 487–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x
- Hilgard, J., Bartholow, B. D., Dickter, C. L., & Blanton, H. (2015). Characterizing switching and congruency effects in the Implicit Association Test as reactive and proactive cognitive control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 381–388. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu060
- Hilgard, J., Weinberg, A., Proudfit, G. H., & Bartholow, B. D. (2014). The negativity bias in affective picture processing depends on bottom-up and top-down motivational significance. Emotion, 14, 940–949. doi: 10.1037/a0036791
- Huang, Y. X., & Luo, Y. J. (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 398, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.074
- Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Bernhardt, J., Dierksmeier, A., & Banse, R. (2011). An inkblot for sexual preference: A semantic variant of the affect misattribution procedure. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 676–690. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.508260
- Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 616–626. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.616
- Junghofer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T. R., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Fleeting images: A new look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology, 38, 175–178. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820175
- Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044
- Kanske, P., Plitschka, J., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Attentional orienting towards emotion: P2 and N400 ERP effects. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3121–3129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.022
- Klauer, K. C., & Mierke, J. (2005). Task-set inertia, attitude accessibility, and compatibility-order effects: New evidence for a task-set switching account of the implicit association test effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 208–217. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271416
- Kristjansson, S. D., Kircher, J. C., & Webb, A. K. (2007). Multilevel models for repeated measures research designs in psychophysiology: An introduction to growth curve modeling. Psychophysiology, 44, 728–736. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00544.x
- Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8.
- Oikawa, M., Aarts, H., & Oikawa, H. (2011). There is a fire burning in my heart: The role of causal attribution in affect transfer. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 156–163. doi: 10.1080/02699931003680061
- Page-Gould, E. (in press). Multilevel modeling. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), The handbook of psychophysiology ( 4th ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Payne, B. K., Brown-Iannuzzi, J., Burkley, M., Arbuckle, N. L., Cooley, E., Cameron, C. D., & Lundberg, K. B. (2013). Intention invention and the affect misattribution procedure: Reply to Bar-Anan and Nosek (2012). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 375–386. doi: 10.1177/0146167212475225
- Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277
- Schapkin, S., Gusev, A., & Kuhl, J. (2000). Categorization of unilaterality presented emotional words: An ERP analysis. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 60, 17–28.
- Scherer, L. D., & Lambert, A. J. (2009). Contrast effects in priming paradigms: Implications for theory and research on implicit attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 383–403. doi: 10.1037/a0015844
- Scherer, L. D., & Schott, J. P. (2012). The affect misattribution task impacts future implicit and explicit judgments. Social Cognition, 30, 537–563. doi: 10.1521/soco.2012.30.5.537
- Schupp, H., Cuthbert, B., Bradley, M., Hillman, C., Hamm, A., & Lang, P. (2003). Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 593–611. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3223-06.2007
- Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257–261. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720257
- Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31–51. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)56002-9
- Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2003). Attention and emotion: An ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing. NeuroReport, 14, 1107–1110. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200306110-00002
- Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2004). The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: An ERP analysis. Psychophysiology, 41(3), 441–449. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00174.x
- Schupp, H. T., Schmälzle, R., & Flaisch, T. (2014). Explicit semantic stimulus categorization interferes with implicit emotion processing. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(11), 1738–1745. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst171
- Schupp, H. T., Schmälzle, R., Flaisch, T., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2013). Reprint of “Affective picture processing as a function of preceding picture valence: An ERP analysis.” Biological Psychology, 92(3), 520–525. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.002
- Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2007). Selective visual attention to emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1082–1089.
- Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., & Presslich, O. (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 23, 695–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x
- Squires, N. K., Squires, K. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1975). Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(4), 387–401.
- Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2006). Interpreting event-related brain potential (ERP) distributions: Implications of baseline potentials and variability with application to amplitude normalization by vector scaling. Biological Psychology, 72, 333–343. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.012
- Weinberg, A., Ferri, J., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Interactions between attention and emotion: Insights from the late positive potential. In M. D. Robinson, E. R. Watkins, & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 35–54). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Beyond good and evil: The time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. Emotion, 10, 767–782. doi: 10.1037/a0020242
- Weinberg, A., Hilgard, J., Bartholow, B. D., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Emotional targets: Evaluative categorization as a function of context and content. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 84, 149–154. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.01.023