“KIN RECOGNITION” AMONG SPADEFOOT TOAD TADPOLES: A SIDE-EFFECT OF HABITAT SELECTION?
David W. Pfennig
Department of Zoology, University of Texas, Austin, TX, 78712-1064 USA
Present address: Department of Zoology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1501 USA.Search for more papers by this authorDavid W. Pfennig
Department of Zoology, University of Texas, Austin, TX, 78712-1064 USA
Present address: Department of Zoology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1501 USA.Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Many animals modify their behavior toward unfamiliar conspecifics as a function of their genetic relatedness. A fundamental problem of any kin recognition study is determining what is being recognized and why. For anuran tadpoles, the predominant view is that associating with relatives is kin-selected because these relatives may thereby accrue benefits through increased growth or predation avoidance. An alternative view is that kin associations are simply a side-effect of habitat selection and thus do not represent attempts to identify kin per se. In the laboratory, spadefoot toad tadpoles (Scaphiopus multiplicatus) preferentially associated with unfamiliar siblings over unfamiliar nonsiblings, as do other anurans. However, same age tadpoles also were more likely to orient toward unfamiliar nonsiblings reared on the same food (familiar food) than toward unfamiliar siblings that were reared on unfamiliar food. These results, together with the results of previous tadpole kin recognition studies, suggest that tadpoles orient toward cues learned early in ontogeny, regardless of the cues' source.
Tadpoles that preferentially associated with cues learned from their environment at birth would tend to be philopatric. Censuses of 14 natural ponds revealed that tadpole density remained greatest near oviposition sites until four days before metamorphosis. Tadpole philopatry may be advantageous: tadpoles restricted to their natal site had greater growth and survivorship than did their siblings restricted to randomly selected sites elsewhere within the same pond. Thus kin affiliative tendency observed in the laboratory in this and perhaps other species of anurans may be a byproduct of habitat selection. Since kin discrimination in animals is most commonly assayed as orientation toward kin, it follows that many examples of “kin recognition” may not represent true attempts to identify kin as such, but rather may reflect some other recognition system that is under entirely different selective pressures.
Literature Cited
- Bateson, P. 1982. Preferences for cousins in Japanese quail. Nature 295: 236–237.
- Beecher, I. M., and M. D. Beecher. 1983. Sibling recognition in bank swallows (Riparia riparia). Z. Tierpsychol. 62: 145–150.
- Beiswenger, R. E. 1975. Structure and function in aggregations of tadpoles of the American toad, Bufo americanus. Herpetologica 31: 222–233.
- Berven K. A. 1981. Altitudinal variation in development and reproduction in the wood frog, Rana sylvatica. Ph.D. Diss. Univ. of Maryland, College Park.
- Black, J. H. 1970. A possible stimulus for the formation of some aggregations in tadpoles of Scaphiopus bombifrons. Proc. Oklahoma Acad. Sci. 49: 13–14.
- Blaustein, A. R. 1988. Ecological correlates and potential functions of kin recognition and kin association in anuran larvae. Behav. Genetics 18: 449–464.
- Blaustein, A. R., M. Bekoff, and T. J. Daniels. 1987a. Kin recognition in vertebrates (excluding primates): Empirical evidence, pp. 287–331. In D. J. C. Fletcher and C. D. Michener (eds.), Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd. N.Y.
- Blaustein, A. R. 1987b. Kin recognition in vertebrates (excluding primates): Mechanisms, functions, and future research, pp. 333–357. In D. J. C. Fletcher and C. D. Michener (eds.), Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd., N.Y.
- Blaustein, A. R., and R. K. O'Hara. 1981. Genetic control for sibling recognition? Nature 290: 246–248.
- Blaustein, A. R. 1982. Kin recognition in Rana cascade tadpoles: Maternal and paternal effects. Anim. Behav. 30: 1151–1157.
- Blaustein, A. R. 1986. An investigation of kin recognition in red-legged frog (Rana aurora) tadpoles. J. Zool. (London), (A) 209: 347–353.
- Blaustein, A. R., R. K. O'Hara, and D. H. Olson. 1984. Kin preference behaviour is retained after metamorphosis in Rana cascade frogs. Anim. Behav. 32: 445–450.
- Bragg, A. N. 1965. Gnomes of the Night: The Spadefoot Toads. Univ. of Pennsylvania Press. Philadelphia.
- Bragg, A. N., and C. King. 1961. Aggregational and associated behavior in tadpoles of the Plains Spadefoot. Wasmann J. Biol. 18: 273–289.
- Breden, F. 1987. The effect of postmetamorphic dispersal on the population genetic structure of Fowler's toad, Bufo woodhousei fowleri. Copeia 1987: 386–395.
10.2307/1445775 Google Scholar
- Breed, M. D., and B. Bennett. 1987. Kin recognition in highly eusocial insects, pp. 243–285. In D. J. C. Fletcher and C. D. Michener (eds.), Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd., N.Y.
- Breed, M. D., L. Butler, and T. M. Stiller. 1985. Kin discrimination by worker honey bees in genetically mixed groups. PNAS, USA 82: 3058–3061.
- Brodie, E. D. Jr., and D. R. Formanowicz Jr. 1987. Antipredator mechanisms of larval anurans: Protection of palatable individuals. Herpetologica 43: 369–373.
- Cheney, D. L., and R. M. Seyfarth. 1982. Recognition of individuals within and between groups of free-ranging vervet monkeys. Am. Zool. 22: 519–530.
- Cornell, T. J., K. A. Berven, and G. C. Gamboa. 1989. Kin recognition by tadpoles and froglets of the wood frog Rana sylvatica. Oecologia 78: 312–316.
- Creusere F. M., and W. G. Whitford. 1976. Ecological relationships in a desert anuran community. Herpetologica 32: 7–18.
- Davis, L. S. 1982. Sibling recognition in Richardson's ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 65–70.
- Dawson, J. T. 1982. Kin recognition and schooling in the American toad (Bufo americanus). Ph.D. Diss. State Univ. of New York, Albany.
- Duellman, W. E., and L. Trueb. 1986. Biology of Amphibians. McGraw Hill, New York.
- Fisher, H. I. 1971. Experiments on homing in Laysan Albatrosses Diomedea immutabilis. Condor 73: 389–400.
- D. J. C. Fletcher, and C. D. Michener (eds.). 1987. Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd., N.Y.
- Foster, M. S., and R. McDiarmid. 1982. Study of aggregative behavior of Rhinophrynus dorsalis tadpoles: Design and analysis. Herpetologica 38: 395–404.
- Fredrickson, W. T., and G. P. Sackett. 1984. Kin preferences in primates (Macaca nemestrina): Relatedness or familiarity? J. Compar. Psychol. 98: 29–34.
- Gascon, C., and C. Planas. 1986. Spring pond water chemistry and the reproduction of the wood frog. Rana sylvatica. Can. J. Zool. 64: 543–550.
- Gosner, K. L. 1960. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16: 183–190.
- Grafen, A. 1990. Do animals really recognize kin? Anim. Behav. 39: 42–54.
- Grau, J. H. 1982. Kin recognition in white-footed deermice (Peromyscus leucopus). Anim. Behav. 30: 497–505.
- Greenberg, L. 1979. Genetic component of bee odor in kin recognition. Science 206: 1095–1097.
- Grosberg, R. K., and J. F. Quinn. 1986. The genetic control and consequences of kin recognition by the larvae of a colonial marine invertebrate. Nature 322: 456–459.
- Grubb, J. C. 1971. Selected aspects of olfactory and visual orientation in anuran amphibians. Ph.D. Diss. Univ. of Texas, Austin.
- Hepper, P. G. 1986. Kin recognition: Functions and mechanisms. A review. Biol. Rev. 61: 63–93.
- Hepper, P. G. 1983. Sibling recognition in the rat. Anim. Behav. 31: 1177–1191.
- Heusser, H. 1969. Die Lebensweise der Erdkröte, Bufo bufo (L.); Das Orientierungsproblem. Rev. Suisse Zool. 76: 443–518.
- Hews, D. K. 1988. Alarm response in larval western toads (Bufo boreas): Release of larval chemicals by a natural predator and its effect on predator capture efficiency. Anim. Behav. 36: 125–133.
- Holmes, W. G. 1984. Sibling recognition in 13-lined ground squirrels: Effects of genetic relatedness, rearing association and olfaction. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 14: 225–233.
- Holmes, W. G., and P. W. Sherman. 1982. The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels. Am. Zool. 22: 491–517.
- Howard, R. D. 1978. The evolution of mating strategies in bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana. Evolution 32: 850–871.
- Howard, R. D. 1980. Mating behaviour and mating success in woodfrogs, Rana sylvatica. Anim. Behav. 28: 705–716.
- Jaenike, J. 1988. Effects of early adult experience on host selection in insects: Some experimental and theoretical results. J. Insect Behav. 1: 3–15.
10.1007/BF01052500 Google Scholar
- Katz, L. C., M. J. Potel, and R. J. Wassersug. 1981. Structure and mechanisms of schooling in tadpoles of the clawed frog, Xenopus laevis. Anim. Behav. 29: 20–33.
- Keough, M. J. 1984. Kin recognition and spatial distribution of larvae of the bryozoan Bugula neritina (L). Evolution 38: 142–147.
- Linsenmair, K. E. 1987. Kin recognition in subsocial arthropods, in particular in the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri, pp. 121–208. In D. J. C. Fletcher and C. D. Michener (eds.), Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd., N.Y.
- Loekle, D. M., D. M. Madison, and J. J. Christian. 1982. Time dependency and kin recognition of cannibalistic behavior among poeciliid fishes. Behav. Neural Biol. 35: 315–318.
- Newman, R. A. 1987. Effects of density and predation on Scaphiopus couchii tadpoles in desert ponds. Oecologia 71: 301–307.
- O'Hara, R. K. 1981. Habitat selection behavior in three species of anuran larvae: Environmental cues, ontogeny, and adaptive significance. Ph.D. Diss. Oregon State Univ., Corvallis.
- O'Hara, R. K., and A. R. Blaustein. 1981. An investigation of sibling recognition in Rana cascade tadpoles. Anim. Behav. 29: 1121–1126.
- O'Hara, R. K. 1982. Kin preference behavior in Bufo boreas tadpoles. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 43–49.
- O'Hara, R. K. 1985. Rana cascade tadpoles aggregate with siblings: an experimental field study. Oecologia 67: 44–51.
- Paterson, H. E. H. 1982. Perspective on speciation by reinforcement. S. Afr. J. Sci. 78: 53–57.
- Pfennig, D. W. 1989. Evolution, development, and behavior of alternative amphibian morphologies. Ph.D. Diss. Univ. Texas, Austin.
- Pfennig, D. W., G. J. Gamboa, H. K. Reeve, J. Shellman-Reeve, and I. D. Ferguson. 1983. The mechanism of nestmate discrimination in social wasps, Polistes (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 13: 299–305.
- Porter, R. H., and M. Wyrick. 1979. Sibling recognition in spiny mice (Acomys cahirinus): Influence of age and isolation. Anim. Behav. 27: 761–766.
- Quinn, T. P., and C. A. Busack. 1985. Chemosensory recognition of siblings in juvenile coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). Anim. Behav. 33: 51–56.
- Radesäter, T. 1976. Individual sibling recognition in juvenile Canada geese (Branta canadensis). Canad. J. Zool. 54: 1069–1072.
- Reeve, H. K. 1989. The evolution of conspecific acceptance thresholds. Am. Nat. 133: 407–435.
- Seale, D. B. 1982. Physical factors influencing oviposition by the woodfrog, Rana sylvatica, in Pennsylvania. Copeia 1982: 627–635.
- Scofield, V. L., J. M. Schlumpberger, L. A. West, and I. L. Weissman. 1982. Protochordate allorecognition is controlled by a MHC-like gene system. Nature 295: 499–502.
- Sherman, P. W., and W. G. Holmes. 1985. Kin recognition: Issues and evidence, pp. 437–460. In B. Hölldobler and M. Lindauer (eds.), Experimental Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA.
- Simovich, M. A., and C. A. Sassaman. 1986. Four independent electrophoretic markers in spadefoot toads. J. Hered. 77: 410–414.
- Waldman, B. 1981. Sibling recognition in toad tadpoles: The role of experience. Z. Tierpsychol. 56: 341–358.
- Waldman, B. 1982. Sibling association among schooling toad tadpoles: Field evidence and implications. Anim. Behav. 30: 700–713.
- Waldman, B. 1984. Kin recognition and sibling association among wood frog (Rana sylvatica) tadpoles. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 14: 171–180.
- Waldman, B. 1985. Olfactory basis of kin recognition in toad tadpoles. J. Comp. Physiol. A 156: 565–577.
- Waldman, B. 1986. Chemical ecology of kin recognition in anuran amphibians, pp. 225–242. In D. Duval, D. Muller-Schwarze, and R. M. Silverstein (eds.), Chemical Signals in Vertebrates. Plenum, N.Y.
- Waldman, B. 1988. The ecology of kin recognition. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 19: 543–571.
- Waldman, B., and K. Adler. 1979. Toad tadpoles associate preferentially with siblings. Nature 282: 611–613.
- Waldman, B., P. C. Frumhoff, and P. W. Sherman. 1988. Problems of kin recognition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 3: 8–13.
- Walters, J. R. 1981. Inferring kinship from behaviour: Maternity determinations in yellow baboons. Anim. Behav 29: 126–136.
- Wassersug, R. J. 1973. Aspects of social behavior in anuran larvae, pp. 273–297. In J. L. Vial. (ed.), Evolutionary Biology of the Anurans. Univ. of Missouri Press, Columbia.
- West-Eberhard, M. J. 1989. Kin recognition in animals. Evolution 43: 703–705.
- Wiens, J. A. 1970. Anuran habitat selection: Early experience and substrate selection in Rana cascade tadpoles. Anim. Behav. 20: 218–221.
- Wilson, E. O. 1987. Kin recognition: An introductory synopsis, pp. 7–18. In D. J. C. Fletcher and C. D. Michener (eds.), Kin Recognition in Animals. John Wiley and Sons Ltd., N.Y.
- Yamazaki, K., E. A. Boyse, V. Mike, H. T. Thaler, B. J. Mathieson, J. Abbot, J. Boyse, Z. A. Zayas, and L. Thomas. 1976. Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the major histocompatibility complex. J. Exp. Med. 144: 1324–1335.