Volume 9, Issue 4 pp. 377-381
Full Access

Foundress behaviors and interactions in polygynous colonies of the haplometrotic Japanese paper wasp Polistes snelleni (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)

Katsuhiko SAYAMA

Katsuhiko SAYAMA

Systematic Entomology, Graduate School of Agriculture, Hokkaido University, Sapporo and Hokkaido Research Center, Forestry and Forest Products Research Institute, Sapporo, Japan

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 21 December 2006
Citations: 3
Katsuhiko Sayama, Hokkaido Research Center, Forestry and Forest Products Research Institute, Sapporo, 062-8516 Japan. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Only two out of 959 pre-emergence colonies of the paper wasp Polistes snelleni de Saussure surveyed between 1989 and 1996 in Sapporo, northern Japan, were found to be two-foundress colonies, and the others were single-foundress colonies. The two foundresses in one of the two colonies showed neither aggressive dominance behavior nor clear division of labor between them during a total of approximately 30 h in the first half of the pre-emergence stage. Although both of the foundresses foraged for pulp and laid eggs, only one foundress foraged for prey and delivered it to the other.

INTRODUCTION

Colonies of Polistes wasps are initiated by a single foundress or multiple foundresses. Most species studied in the genus Polistes show both types of colony founding (Reeve 1991), and the proportion of each type varies from species to species or even from population to population (Itô 1993). However, the reason why these differences occur is not well understood.

In the Japanese Polistes species, the frequency of multiple-female colony founding (pleometrosis) is quite low compared with that in North American and European species, even at similar latitudes (Itô 1993). Why single-female colony founding (haplometrosis) dominates in the Japanese species is a very interesting question, as is the converse question, that is, why does multiple-female colony founding occur only rarely?

There have been several reports on polygynous colonies in Japanese species of the subgenera Polistes (Yamane 1973; Hoshikawa 1979; Makino & Aoki 1982; Kojima & Hagiwara 1990) and Gyrostoma (Yoshikawa 1957; Kasuya 1981; Takamizawa 1982; Hirose & Yamasaki 1984; Tsuchida & Itô 1991). In the subgenus Polistella, only two cases of polygynous colony are known: one in Polistes snelleni de Saussure (Yamane 1969) and the other in Polistes nipponensis Pérez (Y. Sakamoto, unpubl. data, 1993). These reports showed that unlike in the North American and European species, clear division of labor is not found, even if there seems to be dominance behavior among foundresses in these colonies.

Given that multiple-female colony founding is very rare in the Japanese Polistes, analyses of foundress behaviors and interactions in multiple-foundress colonies may help us understand the evolution of single-female colony founding. Here, I report the frequency of polygynous colonies, and foundress behaviors and interactions in polygynous colonies of the Japanese paper wasp P. (Polistella) snelleni.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The study was conducted at Misumai, Sapporo, in northern Japan (42°58′N, 141°16′E) between 1989 and 1996. Every year, as many P. snelleni nests as possible in the egg to early larval substages (the most advanced brood being eggs or young larvae) initiated by overwintered females (foundresses) were found in an area of approximately 8 ha in spring. Nests were usually checked at least every 2 days to determine whether they contained one or more foundresses. In the calculation of the frequency of polygynous colonies, colonies within 1 month after the start of the nesting season were counted.

Foundress behaviors were observed for a total of 29.7 h during the 9 days between 29 May and 18 June in a polygynous colony found in 1993. Behavioral observations were made between 10.00 and 15.00 hours. This period corresponded to the second half of the egg substage and the early larval substage in the development of this colony. Each foundress was marked with quick-drying colored paint for identification. For each foundress, the time and number of take-offs from and landings on the nest were recorded. When foundresses landed on the nest, they were checked to record what kind of load they had brought. Loads observed were prey for food and pulp for nest material. Oviposition, egg-eating (oophagy), and abdomen-rubbing on the nest pedicel or comb surface were also recorded. When both foundresses were on the nest, interactions such as deliveries of collected material, deliveries of regurgitated liquid (trophallaxis), approaches to the other foundress, and touching the other foundress with antennae (antennation) were also noted.

RESULTS

Frequencies of polygynous colonies in Polistes snelleni

A total of 959 colonies were found at the study area in the spring during the 8-year-study period. All except two had a single foundress. Both of the multiple-foundress colonies were two-foundress colonies made on the twigs of shrubs. One two-foundress colony was found in a total of 165 colonies in 1989 and the other two-foundress colony was found in a total of 139 colonies in 1993. The frequency of polygynous colonies was less than 1% in this population as a whole.

Case histories of the two polygynous colonies

I found a polygynous colony with two foundresses on 12 May 1989, when its nest had nine cells, six of which each contained an egg. One of the foundresses was smaller in body size than the other, and had lost her flying ability by early July (pupal substage of the colony) because of damage to her right wing. Although she could not go foraging, she sometimes received prey from the other foundress and fed larvae in the nest. On 28 July, this smaller foundress disappeared from the nest, 2 weeks after the first worker emergence. The larger foundress stayed on the nest until 21 August.

I found the other polygynous colony on 21 May 1993, when its nest had three cells, two of which each contained an egg. Two foundresses were first confirmed to be staying on the same nest with 15 cells and 15 eggs on 28 May. The foundress association in this colony was dissolved early in the larval stage of the colony (20–22 June) because of the disappearance of the more active foundress (foundress A, see below). Although the other foundress maintained the colony until 14–16 August, she also disappeared from the nest without producing any adult offspring of either foundress because most brood were infested by the parasitic wasp Elasmus japonicus Ashmead.

Foundress behavior in one polygynous colony in 1993

In the 1993 polygynous colony, both foundresses foraged and laid eggs. Although there were no significant differences between the foundresses in terms of the performance of several tasks, with the exception of prey collection (two-tailed Mann–Whitney U-test, U = 13.5, P < 0.02, n1 = n2 = 9), one foundress (foundress A) left the nest and worked more frequently than the other (foundress B) (Table 1). Although the foundresses’ body sizes were not measured, there was no apparent difference.

Table 1. Mean number of various behaviors per hour (± SD) on 9 observation days
Behavioral category Mean no. times/h (± SD) (n = 9) Mann–Whitney U-test
Foundress A Foundress B
Foraging trip 1.43 ± 0.98 0.76 ± 0.62 NS
Prey collection 0.21 ± 0.17 0 ± 0 P < 0.02
Pulp collection 0.47 ± 0.54 0.28 ± 0.43 NS
Oviposition 0.16 ± 0.28 0.10 ± 0.15 NS
Oophagy 0.04 ± 0.11 0 ± 0 NS
Rubbing on  pedicel 1.67 ± 1.04 1.81 ± 0.89 NS
Rubbing on  comb 0.23 ± 0.43 0.20 ± 0.31 NS
  • NS, Not significant.

With respect to foraging activity, only foundress A collected insect prey (Table 1). Both foundresses collected pulp, and they made new cells or elongated the existing cell walls. Egg-laying by both foundresses was observed. Foundress A initiated new cells in which the eggs were laid by foundress A and foundress B. Although foundress B initiated one new cell, I did not observe foundress B laying an egg into it. On one occasion, foundress A ate one of foundress B’s eggs just after it had been laid. Although the egg of foundress B that was eaten by foundress A was replaced with a new egg by the next day, I did not observe which female laid the egg.

Abdomen-rubbing behavior is interpreted as a way of applying ant repellent substance (Jeanne 1970; Kojima 1993). No differences were found in rubbing behavior between foundresses; both rubbed not only on the nest pedicel but also on the comb surface. Rubbing on the pedicel was more frequent than on the comb surface (Table 1).

Although the nest was usually attended by one or both foundresses during the observation period, absence of both wasps from the nest sometimes occurred (Fig. 1).

Details are in the caption following the image

Changes in the proportion of time that the nest was attended by foundresses. (●), Foundress A; (○), foundress B; (□), no foundress.

Interactions between the two foundresses in the 1993 nest

When foundress A came back to the nest carrying materials (prey or pulp), she sometimes attempted to deliver these materials to foundress B, who had stayed behind (Table 2), although foundress B did not solicit any materials from foundress A. Approximately half of the times that foundress A attempted to give prey to foundress B, foundress B ran away from the donor rather than receiving the prey. No delivery of pulp between them was observed, despite attempts by foundress A to give it to foundress B (Table 2).

Table 2. Behavioral interactions between the two foundresses in the 1993 nest
Date Most advanced brood No. observation hours No. times accepted/no. times attempted No. times
Delivery of prey Delivery of pulp Trophallaxis Approach Antennation
A to B B to A A to B B to A A to B B to A A to B B to A A to B B to A Mutual
29 May Egg  3.17 0/0 0/0 0/0 0/0  1 0 0 0 0 0 0
31 May Egg  3.20 0/0 0/0 0/0 0/0  2 0 0 1 0 0 0
2 June Egg  3.82 1/1 0/0 0/0 0/0  1 0 0 0 1 1 1
5 June Egg  3.60 0/0 0/0 0/0 0/0  1 1 0 0 0 0 0
11 June Egg  3.78 1/1 0/0 0/1 0/0  4 1 1 0 1 1 1
12 June Egg  2.47 1/1 0/0 0/1 0/0  0 0 1 0 0 0 0
14 June Egg  3.13 0/0 0/0 0/4 0/0  2 0 0 2 0 0 0
17 June Larva  4.05 0/6 0/0 0/0 0/0  1 2 0 0 0 0 0
18 June Larva  2.50 1/1 0/0 0/0 0/0  0 0 0 0 0 0 1
Total 29.72 4/10 0/0 0/6 0/0 12 4 2 3 2 2 3

Trophallaxis was observed more frequently from foundress A to foundress B than from foundress B to foundress A. The two foundresses displayed weak dominance behavior, such as approaches to the other or touching the other with antennae, to an equal extent (Table 2).

DISCUSSION

My results, except for the absence of aggressive dominance behavior between foundresses, are almost the same as those reported for other Japanese Polistes species. The frequency of polygynous colonies was low, and a dominance hierarchy was not established between the foundresses. Although Yamane (1969) reported the presence of one polygynous colony of P. snelleni among more than 100 colonies observed, he could not find any cooperative or dominance behavior between the two foundresses. Behavioral interactions, including domination among foundresses, are important to establish a dominance order and “subordinate workers” (Hunt 1991) (Pardi 1948; West-Eberhard 1969). Makino and Aoki (1982) divided Japanese polygyny into two types on the basis of the presence or absence of domination behavior. According to their classification, domination did not occur in the 1993 polygynous colony in the present study. Regardless of domination, however, oviposition or the development of ovaries occurs in every foundress of the Japanese polygynous colonies (Kasuya 1981; Makino & Aoki 1982). Both foundresses on the studied colony laid eggs during the observation period (Table 1).

Not only in the larval substage but also in the egg substage when no larvae were present in the nest, foundress A collected prey and brought it to the nest. Moreover, after landing on the nest, foundress A attempted to give the prey to foundress B (Table 2). However, foundress A showed reproductive domination by eating one of foundress B’s eggs (Table 1). These behaviors of foundress A are inconsistent with those of dominant females in North American and European species, in which dominant foundresses monopolize oviposition and rarely forage (Pardi 1948; West-Eberhard 1969).

Colony survival (Gibo 1978; Reeve 1991; Itô 1993) and nesting habits (Klahn 1979; Cervo & Turillazzi 1985) have been suggested to be responsible for polygyny or pleometrosis. In contrast, dispersion of foundresses from their natal nest sites (West 1968; Yamane 1986; Suzuki & Ramesh 1992) is thought to promote monogyny or haplometrosis. However, these factors have not been critically examined to show differences in the formation of foundress associations among populations or species.

Why polygynous colonies were formed in only two colonies of P. snelleni is unclear. In this population, colony usurpation frequently occurs in the second half of the pre-emergence stage (Makino & Sayama 1991). Although multiple-female colony founding should be more advantageous in such a situation (Gamboa 1978), its frequency is still low. The abundance of substrates such as twigs and a weak tendency toward philopatry (Makino et al. 1987) may contribute to the maintenance of single-female colony founding. In any case, to understand the maintenance and evolution of single-female colony founding, it is still necessary to gather data on foundress behaviors and interactions in polygynous colonies of the typically haplometrotic Polistes species.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank M. J. West-Eberhard and K. Ozaki for their helpful and critical reading of the manuscript, and Y. Sakamoto for his information on a polygynous colony of Polistes nipponensis.

      The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.