The rating of perceived exertion during competitive running scales with time
James Faulkner
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorGaynor Parfitt
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorRoger Eston
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorJames Faulkner
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorGaynor Parfitt
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorRoger Eston
School of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke's Campus, Exeter, EX1 2LU, United Kingdom
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This study assessed the relationship of the rating of perceived exertion (RPE) with heart rate and pacing strategy during competitive running races of differing distance and course elevation. Nine men and women competed in a 7-mile road race (7-MR) and the Great West Run half marathon (GWR; 13.1 miles). Heart rate, split mile time, and RPE were recorded throughout the races. The RPE was regressed against time and %time to complete the 7-MR and GWR. Although the rate of increase in RPE was greater in the 7-MR, there were no differences when expressed against %time (inferring that the brain uses a scalar timing mechanism). As the course elevation, distance, pacing strategy, and heart rate response varied between conditions, this study has provided evidence that the perceptual response may have distinct temporal characteristics during distance running. The results provide further evidence that RPE scales with the proportion of exercise time that remains.
REFERENCES
- Albertus, Y., Tucker, R., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., Hampson, D. B., & Noakes, T. D. (2005). Effect of distance feedback on pacing strategies and perceived exertion during cycling. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 37, 461–468.
- Atkinson, G., Peacock, O., St Clair Gibson, A., & Tucker, R. (2007). Distribution of power output during cycling: Impact and implications. Sports Medicine, 37, 647–667.
- Baden, D. A., Warwick-Evans, L., & Lakomy, J. (2004). Am I nearly there? The effect of anticipated running distance on perceived exertion and attentional focus. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 1–17.
- Baldwin, J., Snow, R. J., Gibala, M. J., Garnham, A., Howarth, K., & Febbraio, M. A. (2003). Glycogen availability does not affect the TCA cycle or TAN pools during prolonged fatiguing exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 94, 2181–2187.
- Bishop, D., Bonetti, D., & Dawson, B. (2002). The influence of pacing strategy on VO2 and supramaximal kayak performance. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 6, 1041–1047.
- Borg, G. (1998). Borg's perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
- Cafarelli, E. (1982). Peripheral contributions to the perception of effort. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 14, 382–389.
- Cafarelli, E., & Noble, B. J. (1976). The effect of inspired carbon dioxide on subjective estimates of exertion during exercise. Ergonomics, 19, 581–589.
- Catalano, J. F. (1973). Effect of perceived proximity to end of task upon end-spurt. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 363–377.
- Catalano, J. F. (1974). End-spurt following simple repetitive muscular movement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 763–766.
- Cooke, C. B. (2001). Maximal oxygen uptake, economy and efficiency. In R. G. Eston & T. Reilly (Eds), Kinanthropometry and Exercise Physiology Laboratory Manual: Tests, Procedures and Data. Vol. 2: Exercise physiology (pp. 161–191). London: Routledge.
- Crewe, H., Tucker, R., & Noakes, T. D. (2008). The rate of increase in rating of perceived exertion predicts the duration of exercise to fatigue at a fixed power output in different environmental conditions. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 103, 569–577.
- De Koning, J. J., Bobbert, M. F., & Foster, C. (1999). Determination of optimal pacing strategy in track cycling with an energy flow model. Journal of Science, Medicine and Sport, 2, 266–277.
- Eston, R. G., Faulkner, J. A., St Clair Gibson, A., Noakes, T., & Parfitt, G. (2007). The effect of antecedent fatiguing activity on the relationship between perceived exertion and physiological activity during a constant load exercise task. Psychophysiology, 44, 779–786.
- Foster, C., Schrager, M., Snyder, A. C., & Thompson, N. N. (1994). Pacing strategy and athletic performance. Sports Medicine, 17, 77–85.
- Hampson, D. B., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. I., & Noakes, T. D. (2001). The influence of sensory cues on the performance of effort during exercise and central regulation of exercise performance. Sports Medicine, 31, 935–952.
- Horstman, D. H., Morgan, W. P., Cymerman, A., & Stokes, J. (1979). Perception of effort during constant work to self-imposed exhaustion. Perceptual Motor Skills, 48, 1111–1126.
- Jones, A. M. (2007). Middle and long distance running. In E. M. Winter, A. M. Jones, R. R. C. Davidson, P. D. Bromley, & T. H. Mercer (Eds.), Sport and exercise physiology testing guidelines; The British Association of Sport and Exercise Sciences Guide: Vol. 1, Sport Testing (pp. 147–154). London: Routeledge.
- Jones, A. M., & Doust, J. H. (1996). A 1% treadmill grade most accurately reflects the energetic cost of outdoor running. Journal of Sport Sciences, 14, 321–327.
- Joseph, T., Johnson, B., Battista, R. A., Wright, G., Dodge, C., Porcari, J. P., et al. (2008). Perception of fatigue during simulated competition. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 40, 381–386.
- Kay, D., Marino, F. E., Cannon, J., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. I., & Noakes, T. D. (2001). Evidence for neuromuscular fatigue during cycling in warm humid conditions. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 84, 115–121.
- Lambert, E. V., St Clair Gibson, A., & Noakes, T. D. (2005). Complex system model of fatigue: Integrative homeostatic control of peripheral physiological systems during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 52–62.
- Miyake, Y., Onishi, Y., & Poppel, E. (2004). Two types of anticipation in synchronization tapping. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 415–426.
- Morgan, W. P., & Borg, G. A. V. (1976). Perception of effort in the prescription of physical activity. In T. Craig (Ed.), The humanistic and mental health aspects of sports, exercise and recreation (pp. 126–129). Chicago: American Medical Association.
- Noakes, T. D. (2000). Physiological models to understand exercise fatigue and the adaptations that predict or enhance athletic performance. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine Science and Sports, 10, 123–145.
- Noakes, T. D. (2004). Linear relationship between the perception of effort and the duration of constant load exercise that remains. Journal of Applied Physiology, 96, 1571–1573.
- Noakes, T. D., St Clair Gibson, A., & Lambert, E. V. (2004). From catastrophe to complexity: A novel model of integrative neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 511–514.
- Noble, B. J., & Robertson, R. J. (1997). Perceived exertion. Leeds: Human Kinetics.
- Pandolf, K. B., Burse, R. L., & Goldman, R. F. (1975). Differentiated ratings of perceived exertion during physical conditioning of older individuals using leg-weight loading. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 40, 563–574.
- Pennebaker, J. W., & Lightner, J. M. (1980). Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 165–174.
- Rauch, H. G. L., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., & Noakes, T. D. (2005). A signalling role for muscle glycogen in the regulation of pace during prolonged exercise. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 34–38.
- Rejeski, W. J. (1985). The perception of exertion: An active or passive process? Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 371–378.
-
Rejeski, W. J., & Ribisil, P. M. (1980). Expected task duration and perceived effort: An attributional analysis.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 227–236.
10.1123/jsp.2.3.227 Google Scholar
- Robertson, R. J., Falkel, J. E., Drash, A. L., Swank, A. M., Metz, K. F., Spungen, S. A., et al. (1986). Effect of blood pH on peripheral and central signals of perceived exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 18, 114–122.
- Staab, J. S., Agnew, J. W., & Siconolfi, S. F. (1992). Metabolic and performance responses to uphill and downhill running in distance runners. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 24, 124–127.
- St Clair Gibson, A., Baden, D. A., Lambert, M. I., Lambert, E. V., Harley, Y. X. R., Hampson, D., et al. (2003). The conscious perception of the sensation of fatigue. Sports Medicine, 33, 167–176.
- St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., Rauch, L. H. G., Tucker, R., Baden, D. A., Foster, C., et al. (2006). The role of information processing between the brain and peripheral physiological systems in pacing and perception of effort. Sports Medicine, 36, 705–722.
- St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. I., & Noakes, T. D. (2001). Neural control of force output during maximal and submaximal exercise. Sports Medicine, 31, 637–650.
- St Clair Gibson, A., & Noakes, T. D. (2004). Evidence for complex system integration and dynamic neural regulation of skeletal muscle recruitment during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 797–806.
- Thomas, J. R., & Nelson, J. K. (1996). Research methods in physical activity. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
- Tucker, R., Rauch, L. G. H., Harley, Y. X., & Noakes, T. D. (2004). Impaired exercise performance in heat is associated with an anticipatory reduction in skeletal muscle recruitment. European Journal of Physiology, 48, 422–430.
- Ulmer, H. V. (1996). Concept of an extracellular regulation of muscular metabolic rate during heavy exercise in humans by psychophysiological feedback. Experientia, 52, 416–420.
- Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgements: An executive controlled perspective. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 319–328.