Believing that prejudice can change increases children's interest in interracial interactions
Corresponding Author
Kristin Pauker
Department of Psychology, University of Hawai‘i at Manoa, Honolulu, Hawaii, USA
Correspondence
Kristin Pauker, Department of Psychology, 2530 Dole Street, Sakamaki C400, University of Hawai‘i at Manoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorEvan P. Apfelbaum
Questrom School of Business, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
Search for more papers by this authorCarol S. Dweck
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
Search for more papers by this authorJennifer L. Eberhardt
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Kristin Pauker
Department of Psychology, University of Hawai‘i at Manoa, Honolulu, Hawaii, USA
Correspondence
Kristin Pauker, Department of Psychology, 2530 Dole Street, Sakamaki C400, University of Hawai‘i at Manoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Search for more papers by this authorEvan P. Apfelbaum
Questrom School of Business, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
Search for more papers by this authorCarol S. Dweck
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
Search for more papers by this authorJennifer L. Eberhardt
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Children begin interacting less across racial lines around middle childhood, but it remains unclear why. We examine the novel possibility that, at that time, children's prejudice theories—their understanding of prejudice as a fixed or malleable attribute—begin to influence their desire for interracial affiliation. We devise immersive behavioral experiences to evaluate when and how prejudice theories affect interracial affiliation. Study 1 measured prejudice theories among 8–13-year-olds (N = 152; 76 White, 76 racial minority) and observed children in a newly-developed social interaction task. In line with our hypothesis, children older than 10 years with stronger malleable-prejudice theories exhibited more interest and affiliation in a simulated cross- (vs. same-race) interaction, regardless of their preexisting prejudice level. Study 2 randomly assigned children to listen to a fixed- or malleable-prejudice theory story before engaging in a real, first-time interaction with a same- or cross-race partner at a different school via live video-stream (N = 150; 96 White, 54 racial minority). The malleable theory increased children's interest in further interaction with their cross-race partner. These findings highlight the promise of malleable-prejudice theories for sustaining positive interracial relationships during a critical developmental window—when the frequency of cross-race friendships typically declines.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None to declare.
Open Research
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
desc13233-sup-0001-SuppMat.docx65.7 KB | Supporting information. |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
REFERENCES
- Aboud, F. E., Mendelson, M. J., & Purdy, K. T. (2003). Cross-race peer relations and friendship quality. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000164
- Aboud, F. E., Tredoux, C., Tropp, L. R., Brown, C. S., Niens, U., & Noor, N. M., & the Una Evaluation Group (2012). Interventions to reduce prejudice and enhance inclusion and respect for ethnic differences in early childhood: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 32, 307–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.05.001
- Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage.
- Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. In ( M. Zanna Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol., 32, pp. 201–271). Academic Press.
- Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Learning (not) to talk about race: When older children underperform in social categorization. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1513–1518. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012835
- Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R., & Ambady, N. (2010). In blind pursuit of racial equality? Psychological Science, 21, 1587–1592. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610384741
- Babbitt, L. G., & Sommers, S. R. (2011). Framing matters: Contextual influences on interracial interaction outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1233–1244. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211410070
- Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Coordinated movement in human interaction. In ( R.S. Feldman & B. Rime ́ Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432). Cambridge University Press.
- Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x
- Brown, C. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2005). Children's perceptions of discrimination: A developmental model. Child Development, 76(3), 533–553. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00862.x
- Carr, P. B., Dweck, C. S., & Pauker, K. (2012). Prejudiced” behavior without prejudice? Beliefs about the malleability of prejudice affect interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 452–471. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028849
- Davies, K., Tropp, L. R., Aron, A., Pettigrew, T. F., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Cross-group friendships and intergroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 332–351. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411103
- Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic prejudice in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 356–374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017993
- Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 62–68. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.62
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 248–253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.006
- Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. Guilford Press.
- Fisher, C. B., Wallace, S. A., & Fenton, R. E. (2000). Discrimination distress during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 679–695. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026455906512
- Fitzroy, S., & Rutland, A. (2010). Learning to control ethnic intergroup bias in childhood. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 679–693. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.746
- Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 91–107. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.91
- Golarai, G., Ghahremani, D. G., Greenwood, A. C., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Eberhardt, J. E. (2021). The development of race effects in face processing from childhood through adulthood: Neural and behavioral evidence. Developmental Science, 24, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13058
- Gonzalez, A. M., Steele, J. R., & Baron, A. S. (2017). Reducing children's implicit racial bias through exposure to positive outgroup exemplars. Child Development, 88(1), 123–130. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12582
- Gonzalez, A. M., Steele, J. R., Chan, E. F., Lim, S. A., & Baron, A. S. (2021). Developmental differences in the malleability of implicit racial bias following exposure to counterstereotypical exemplars. Developmental Psychology, 57(1), 102–113. http://doi.org/10.1037/dev000112
- Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
- Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575
- Heck, R. H., Thomas, S. L., & Tabata, L. N. (2012). Multilevel modeling of categorical outcomes using IBM SPSS. Routledge Academic. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203808986
- Jugert, P., Noack, P., & Rutland, A. (2013). Children's cross-ethnic friendships: Why are they less stable than same-ethnic friendships? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 649–662. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.734136.
- Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., Murrar, S., Brauer, M., Devos, T., Calanchini, J., Xiao, Y. J., Pedram, C., Marshburn, C. K., Simon, S., Blanchar, J. C., Joy-Gaba, J. A., Conway, J., Redford, L., Klein, R. A., Roussos, G., Schellhaas, F. M. H., Burns, M., …, & Nosek, B. A. (2016). Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 1001–1016. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000179
- Lessard, L. M., Kogachi, K., & Juvonen, J. (2019). Quality and stability of cross-ethnic friendships: Effects of classroom diversity and out-of-school contact. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48, 554–566. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0964-9
- Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). The impact of children's static vs. dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype formation. Child Development, 70, 1163–1180. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00085
- Mallett, R. K., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Expect the unexpected: Failure to anticipate similarities when predicting the quality of an intergroup interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 265–277. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.94.2.265
- McCauley, C., Plummer, M., Moskalenko, S., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2001). The exposure index: A measure of intergroup contact. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 7, 321–336. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327949PAC0704_03
10.1207/S15327949PAC0704_03 Google Scholar
- McKown, C. (2004). Age and ethnic variation in children's thinking about the nature of racism. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 597–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.001
- Migacheva, K., & Tropp, L. R. (2013). Learning orientation as a predictor of positive intergroup contact. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16, 426–444. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430212455854
- Murphy, M. C., Richeson, J. A., & Molden, D. C. (2011). Leveraging motivational mindsets to foster positive interracial interactions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 118–131. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00338.x
10.1111/j.1751?9004.2010.00338.x Google Scholar
- Neel, R., & Shapiro, J. R. (2012). Is racial bias malleable? Whites' lay theories of racial bias predict divergent strategies for interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 101–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028237
- Pauker, K., Apfelbaum, E. P., & Spitzer, B. (2015). The race talk dilemma for racial minority children. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 887–895. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615598379
- Pauker, K., Williams, A., & Steele, J. R. (2017). The development of racial categorization in childhood. In ( A. Rutland, D. Nesdale, & C. S. Brown Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of group processes in children and adolescents (pp. 221–229). John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
10.1002/9781118773123.ch11 Google Scholar
- Pearson, A. R., West, T. V., Dovidio, J. F., Powers, S. R., Buck, R., & Henning, R. (2008). The fragility of intergroup relations: Divergent effects of temporal delay in audio-visual feedback in intergroup and intragroup interaction. Psychological Science, 19, 1272–1279. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02236.x.
- Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2003). The antecedents and implications of interracial anxiety. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 790–801. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029006011
- Qian, M. K., Heyman, G. D., Quinn, P. C., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2019). Differential developmental courses of implicit and explicit biases for different other-race classes. Developmental Psychology, 55(7), 1440–1452. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000730
- Qian, M. K., Quinn, P. C., Heyman, G. D., Pascalis, O., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2017). Perceptual individuation training (but not mere exposure) reduces implicit racial bias in preschool children. Developmental psychology, 53(5), 845–859. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000290
- Qian, M. K., Quinn, P. C., Heyman, G. D., Pascalis, O., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2019). A long-term effect of perceptual individuation training on reducing implicit racial bias in preschool children. Child Development, 90(3), e290–e305. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12971
- Quintana, S. M. (2008). Racial perspective taking ability: Developmental, theoretical, and empirical trends. In ( S. M. Quintana & C. McKown Eds.), Handbook of race, racism, and the developing child (pp. 16–36). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118269930.ch2
- Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 1715–1737. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x
- Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 316–320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00528.x
- Rogers, L. O., Rosario, R. J., Padilla, D., & Foo, C. (2021). [I]t's hard because it's the cops that are killing us for stupid stuff”: Racial identity in the sociopolitical context of Black Lives Matter. Developmental Psychology, 57(1), 87–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001130
- Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., & McGeorge, P. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children's implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76, 451–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2005.00856.x
- Shelton, J. N. (2000). A reconceptualization of how we study issues of racial prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 374–390. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0404_6
- Shelton, J. N., Richeson, J. A., & Bergsieker, H. B. (2009). Interracial friendship development and attributional biases. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26, 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407509106707
- Shrum, W., Cheek, N. H., & Hunter, S. M. (1988). Friendship in school: Gender and racial homophily. Sociology of Education, 61, 227–239. https://doi.org/10.2307/2112441
- Steele, J. R., George, M., Williams, A., & Tay, E. (2018). A cross-cultural investigation of children's implicit attitudes toward White and Black racial outgroups. Developmental Science, 21, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12673
- Sullivan, J. N., Eberhardt, J. L., & Roberts, S. O. (2021). Conversations about race in Black and White US families: Before and after George Floyd's death. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(38), e2106366118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2106366118
- Tackett, J. L., Herzhoff, K., Kushner, S. C., & Rule, N. (2016). Thin slices of child personality: Perceptual, situational, and behavioral contributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 150–166. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000044.
- Tickle-Degnan, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285–293. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1
10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1 Google Scholar
- Toosi, N. R., Babbitt, L. G., Ambady, N., & Sommers, S. R. (2012). Dyadic interracial interactions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025767
- Trawalter, S., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2009). Predicting behavior during interracial interactions: A stress and coping approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 243–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309345850
- Tuncgenc, B., & Cohen, E. (2016). Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00782
- Wölfer, R., Schmid, K., Hewstone, M., & van Zalk, M. (2016). Developmental dynamics of intergroup contact and intergroup attitudes: Long-term effects in adolescence and early adulthood. Child Development, 87(5), 1466–1478. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12598