Does why we reminisce reflect how you behave? Linking maternal reminiscing goals to child behavioural and emotional problems
Corresponding Author
Sarah Kulkofsky
Department of Human Development & Family Studies, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
Department of Human Development & Family Studies, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA===Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Sarah Kulkofsky
Department of Human Development & Family Studies, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
Department of Human Development & Family Studies, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA===Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Recent theoretical and empirical work suggests that joint mother–child reminiscence may reflect both explicit and implicit socialization goals. The present study investigates mothers' explicit socialization goals that may be enacted during joint reminiscence in relation to children's behavioural and emotional problems. Two hundred and sixty-five mothers of 3- to 8-year-old children completed an online survey that included a measure of seven reminiscing goals (emotional understanding, behaviour control, bonding, interdependence, entertainment, memory skill development, and maintaining peer relationships). In addition, mothers completed measures of children's emotion problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity, and peer problems as well as supportive parenting and maternal attachment. Findings indicated that controlling for supportive parenting and maternal attachment, emotional understanding was a unique predictor of children's emotion problems, directing behaviour was a unique predictor of conduct problems, and bonding was a unique predictor of hyperactivity. These findings provide support for the functional nature of joint reminiscence. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
REFERENCES
- Alexander, K. W., Quas, J. A., & Goodman, G. S. (2002). Theoretical advances in understanding children's memory for distressing events: The role of attachment. Developmental Review, 22, 490–519.
- Armstrong, M. I., Birnie-Lefcovitch, S., & Ungar, M. T. (2005). Pathways between social support, family well being, quality of parenting, and child resilience: What we know. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 14, 269–281.
10.1007/s10826-005-5054-4 Google Scholar
- Berg-Nielsen, T. S., Vikan, A., & Dahl, A. A. (2002). Parenting related to child and parental psychopathology: A descriptive review of the literature. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 7, 529–552.
10.1177/1359104502007004006 Google Scholar
- Bird, A., Reese, E., & Tripp, G. (2006). Parent-child talk about past emotional events: Associations with child temperament and goodness-of-fit. Journal of Cognition & Development, 7, 189–210.
- Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (2002). Exploring the functions of autobiographical memory: Why do I remember the autumn? In J. D. Webster & B. K. Haight (Eds.), Critical advances in reminiscence work: From theory to application (pp. 61–75). New York: Springer.
- Bluck, S., Alea, N., Habermas, T., & Rubin, D. C. (2005). A tale of three functions: The self-reported uses of autobiographical memory. Social Cognition, 23, 91–117.
- Bridges, L. J. (2003). Trust, attachment, and relatedness. In M. H. Bornstein, L. Davidson, C. L. M. Keyes, & K. A. Moore (Eds.), Well-being: Positive development across the life course (pp. 177–189). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
- Bruce, D. (1989). Functional explanations of memory. In L. C. Poon, D. C. Rubin, & B. A. Wilson (Eds.), Everyday cognition in adulthood and late life (pp. 44–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Campbell, S. B. (1997). Behavior problems in preschool children: Developmental and family issues. Advances in Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 1–26.
- Cox, M. J., & Harter, K. S. M. (2003). Parent-child relationships. In M. H. Bornstein, L. Davidson, C. L. M. Keyes, & K. A. Moore (Eds.), Well-being: Positive development across the life course (pp. 191–204). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
- Fivush, R., Reese, E., & Haden, C. A. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588.
- Fivush, R., & Sales, J. M. (2006). Coping, attachment, and mother–child narratives of stressful events. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 125–150.
- Fivush, R., & Vasudeva, A. (2002). Remembering to relate: Socioemotional correlates of mother–child reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 73–90.
- Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 350–365.
- Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.
- Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337–1345.
- Hudson, J. A. (1990). The emergence of autobiographic memory in mother–child conversation. In R. Fivush, & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 166–196). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Hyman, I. E., & Faries, J. M. (1992). The functions of autobiographical memory. In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory. The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
- Kleinbaum, D. G., Kupper, L. L., Muller, K. E., & Nizam, A. (1998). Applied regression analysis and multivariate methods. Albany, NY: Duxbury Press.
- Kulkofsky, S., & Koh, J. B. K. (2009). Why they reminiscence: Caregiver reports of the functions of joint reminiscence in early childhood. Memory, 17, 458–470.
- Kulkofsky, S., Wang, Q., & Koh, J. B. K. (2009). Functions of memory sharing and mother–child reminiscing behaviors: Individual and cultural variations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 92–114.
- Laible, D. J. (2004). Mother–child discourse surrounding a child's past behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 159–181.
- Lewis, K. D. (1999). Maternal style in reminiscing: Relations to child individual differences. Cognitive Development, 14, 381–399.
- Lovejoy, M. C., Weis, R., O'Hare, E., & Rubin, E. (1999). Development and initial validation of the Parent Behavior Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 11, 534–545.
- Lyons-Ruth, K. (2008). Contributions of the mother–infant relationship to dissociative, borderline, and conduct symptoms in young adulthood. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29, 203–218.
- McLean, K. C. (2005). Late adolescent identity development: Narrative meaning making and memory telling. Developmental Psychology, 41, 683–691.
- Neisser, U. (1988). The ecological approach to perception and memory. New Trends in Experimental and Clinical Psychiatry, 4, 153–166.
- Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 7–14.
- Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511.
- Newcombe, R., & Reese, E. (2004). Evaluations and orientations in mother–child narratives as a function of attachment security: A longitudinal investigation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 230–245.
- Pillemer, D. B. (1992). Remembering personal circumstances: A functional analysis. In E. Winograd, & U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of ‘flashbulb’ memories (pp. 236–264). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Reese, E. (2008). Maternal coherence in the Adult Attachment Interview is linked to maternal reminiscing and to children's self concept. Attachment & Human Development, 10, 451–464.
- Reese, E., & Farrant, K. (2003). Social origins of reminiscing. In R. Fivush, & C. A. Haden (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 29–48). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Sales, J. M., & Fivush, R. (2005). Social and emotional functions of mother–child reminiscing about stressful events. Social Cognition, 23, 70–90.
- Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). What do self-report attachment measures assess? In W. S. Rholes, & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 17–54). New York: Guilford.
- Stoops, N. (2004). Educational attainment in the United States: 2003. Current Population Reports No. P20–550. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce.
- Thompson, A., Hollis, C., & Richards, D. (2003). Authoritarian parenting attitudes as a risk for conduct problems: Results from a British national cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 84–91.
- United States Census Bureau (2007). Annual estimates of the population by sex, race, and hispanic origin for the United States: April 1, 2000 to July 1, 2007 (NC-EST2007-03). Retrieved May 15, 2009, from http://www.census.gov/popest/national/asrh/NC-EST2007-srh.html.
- Wang, Q. (2004). The cultural context of parent-child reminiscing: A functional analysis. In M. W. Pratt, & B. H. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations (pp. 279–301). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Wang, Q. (2006). Relations of maternal style and child self-concept to autobiographical memories in Chinese, Chinese Immigrant, and European American 3-year-olds. Child Development, 77, 1794–1809.
- Wareham, P., & Salmon, K. (2006). Mother–child reminiscing about everyday experiences: Implications for psychological interventions in the preschool years. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 535–554.
- Webster, J. D. (2003). The reminiscence circumplex and autobiographical memory functions. Memory, 11, 203–215.