Depression and social comparison motives
Corresponding Author
Gifford Weary
Ohio State University
Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, 1885 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA or Kerry Marsh, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, 46556Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Gifford Weary
Ohio State University
Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, 1885 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA or Kerry Marsh, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, 46556Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This study examined the role of informational social comparison motives in depressed and nondepressed individuals' opinion comparison activities. In particular. We examined the impact of agreement and disagreement from sources similar or dissimilar to depressed and nondepressed subjects on an attribute related to the focal judgment. AS predicted, depressed compared to nondepressed subjects indicated a greater preference for the similar-disagreer, whereas nondepressed preferred as a partner the dissimilar-agreer to a greater extent than did depressed subjects. Furthermore, measures of validation and construction motives were found to be associated with diflerent partner preferences. Results are discussed in terms of the multiple motivations underlying and distinguishing depressed and nondepressed social comparison activities.
References
- Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. and Teasdale, J. P. (1978). ‘Learned helplessness: Critique and reformulation’, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87: 49–74.
- Alloy, L. B. and Abramson, L. Y. (1988). ‘ Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives’ In: L. B. Alloy (Ed.) Cognitive Processes in Depression, Guilford, New York, pp. 223–265.
- Alloy, L. B. and Ahrens, A. (1987). ‘Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self versus others’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52: 366–318.
- Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and Treatment, University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia.
- Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders, International Universities Press, New York.
- Brewin, C. R. and Furnham, A. (1986). ‘Attributional versus preattributional variables in self-esteem and depression: A comparison and test of learned helplessness theory’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50: 1012–1020.
- Crocker, J., Alloy, L. B. and Kayne. N. T. (1988). ‘Attributional style, depression, and perceptions of consensus for events’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54: 840–846.
- Crocker, J., Kayne. N. T. and Alloy, L. B. (1985). ‘Comparing the self with others in depressed and nondepressed college students: Reply to McCauley’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48: 1579–1583.
- Fazio, R. H. (1979). ‘Motives for social comparison: The construction-validation distinction’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37: 1683–1698.
- Festinger. L. (1954). ‘A theory of social comparison processes’, Human Relations, 7: 117–140.
- Fletcher, G. J. O., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Peterson, D. and Reeder, G. D. (1986). ‘Attributional complexity: An individual differences measure’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 975–984.
- Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., Pliner, P. and Krames, L. (1987). ‘Depression and social comparison information-seeking’, Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2: 473–484.
- Forgas, J. P. (1991). ‘Affective influences in partner choice: Role of mood in social decisions’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61: 708–120.
- Gibbons, F. X. (1986). ‘Social comparison and depression: Company's effect on misery’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 140–148.
- Gleicher, F. and Weary, G. (1991). ‘The effect of depression on the quantity and quality of social inferences’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61: 105–114.
- Goethals, G. R. and Darley, J. M. (1977). ‘ Social comparison theory: An attributional approach’ In: J. M. Suls and R. L. Miller (Eds). Social Comparlson Processes: Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives, Hemisphere, Washington, D.C., pp. 259–278.
- Lewinsohn, P. M., Hoberman. H. M. and Rosenbaum, M. A. (1988). ‘Prospective study of risk factors for unipolar depression’, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97: 251–264.
- Locke, K. D. and Horowitz, L. M. (1990). ‘Satisfaction in interpersonal interactions as a function of similarity in level of dysphoria’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58: 823–831.
- Marks, G. and Miller, N. (1987). ‘Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review’, Psychological Bulletin, 102: 72–90.
- Marsh, K. L. and Weary, G. (in press). Severity of depression and responsiveness to attributional information. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology.
- Marsh, K. L. and Weary, G. (1989). Depression and attributional complexity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15: 325–336.
- Martin, D. J., Abramson, L. and Alloy, L. B. (1984). ‘Illusion of control for self and others In depressed and nondepressed college students’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46: 125–136.
- Rosenblatt, A. and Greenberg, J. (1988). ‘Depression and interpersonal attraction: The role of perceived similarity’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60: 620–62.
- Rosenblatt, A. and Greenberg, J. (1991). ‘Examining the world of the depressed: Do depressed people prefer others who are depressed?’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60: 620–629.
- Sacco, W. P. (1981). ‘Invalid use of the Beck Depression Inventory to identify depressed college student Subjects: A methodological comment’, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 5: 143–14.
- Tabachnik, N., Crocker, J. and Alloy, L. B. (1983). ‘Depression, social comparison, and the false-consensus effect’, Journal of Personolity and Social Psychology, 45: 688–69.
- Warren, L. W. and McEachren, L. (1983). ‘Psychological correlates of depressive symptomatology in adult women’, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92: 151–160.
- Weary, G. and Edwards, J. A. (1993). ‘ Social cognition and clinical psychology: Anxiety, depression and the processing of social information’ In: R. S. Wyer and T. K. Srull (Eds). Handbook of Social Cognition, Vol. 2, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
- Weary, G., Elbin, S. and Hill, M. G. (1987). ‘Attributional and social comparison processes in depression’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52: 605–610
- Weary, G., Hill, M. G. and Jordan, J. S. (1984). ‘Observers' reactions to attributors' ascriptions of causal responsibility. Concordance of causal understanding’, Social Psychology Quarterly, 47: 102–106.
-
Weary, G.,
Marsh, K. L.,
Gleicher, F. and
Edwards, J. A.
(1993).
‘ Depression, control motivation, and the processing of information about others’ In:
G. Weary,
F. Gleicher and
K. L. Marsh (Eds).
Control Motivation and Social Cognition,
Springer-Verlag,
New York.
10.1007/978-1-4613-8309-3 Google Scholar
- Wenzlaff, R. M. and Prohaska, M. L. (1989). ‘When misery prefers company: Depression, attributions, and responses to others' moods’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25: 220–233.
- Wheeler, L. and Miyake, K. (1992). ‘Social comparison in everyday life’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62: 760–773.