Hash browns for breakfast, baked potatoes for dinner: Changes in food attitudes as a function of motivation and context
Corresponding Author
Shelley N. Aikman
Syracuse University, USA
Department of Psychology, 430 Huntington Hall, Syracuse University, Syracuse NY 13244, USA.Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Shelley N. Aikman
Syracuse University, USA
Department of Psychology, 430 Huntington Hall, Syracuse University, Syracuse NY 13244, USA.Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Two studies investigated whether participants' motivational state and the context in which attitude reports are made influence food attitudes. Specifically, these studies examined whether hunger and the time-typicality of foods (i.e. match or mismatch between the time when a food is typically eaten and the time the attitude is reported) interact to influence reported attitudes. Study 1 suggests that hunger leads to more positive attitudes toward foods that are typically eaten at the time the attitude report is made (e.g. breakfast foods in morning) compared to foods not typically eaten at the time the attitude report is made (e.g. breakfast foods in evening). Study 2 replicates this time-typical effect of hunger and suggests that time-typical experience rather than general experience with foods is important for hunger induced attitude change. By demonstrating that food attitudes are influenced by motivational states and the match between when the attitude is reported and when it is typically encountered, the present studies extend previous attitude theory and research that has identified other contextual factors that influence attitude reports. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
REFERENCES
- Abelson, R. P., Kinder, D. R., Peters, M. D., & Fiske, S. T. (1982). Affective and semantic components in political person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 619–630.
- Aikman, S. N., Crites, S. L., Jr., & Fabrigar, L. R. (2003). Beyond affect and cognition: Identification of the informational bases of food attitudes. Manuscript submitted for publication.
- Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on behavior. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 385–403). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (1999). Distinguishing perceptions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting consumption of a low-fat diet using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 72–90.
- Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2000). Attitudinal ambivalence: A test of three key hypotheses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1421–1432.
- Blascovich, J., Ernst, J. M., Tomaka, J., Kelsey, R. M., Salomon, K. L., & Fazio, R. H. (1993). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of autonomic reactivity during decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 165–176.
- Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401–423.
- Candel, M. J. J. M. (2001). Consumers' convenience orientation towards meal preparation: Conceptualization and measurement. Appetite, 36, 15–28.
- Edwards, K. (1990). The interplay of affect and cognition in attitude formation and change. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 59, 202–216.
- Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (1999). The role of the affective and cognitive bases of attitudes in susceptibility to affectively and cognitively based persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 363–381.
- Fazio, R. H., & Powell, M. C. (1997). On the value of knowing one's likes and dislikes: Attitude accessibility, stress, and health in college. Psychological Science, 8, 430–436.
- Fazio, R. H., Blascovich, J., & Driscoll, D. M. (1992). On the functional value of attitudes: The influence of accessible attitudes on the ease and quality of decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 388–401.
- Fazio, R. H., Ledbetter, J. E., & Towless-Schwen, T. (2000). On the costs of accessible attitudes: Detecting that the attitude object has changed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 197–210.
- Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). The constructive nature of automatic evaluation. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 169–188). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1980). Restrained eating. In A. J. Stunkard (Ed.), Obesity (pp. 208–225). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
- Hox, J. J. (1995). Applied multilevel analyses. Amsterdam: TT-Publikaties.
- Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2001). Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subject designs. Psychological Methods, 6(2), 115–134.
- Kreft, I., & de Leeuw, J. (1998). Introducing multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
10.4135/9781849209366 Google Scholar
- Kreft, I. G. G., de Leeuw, J. D., & Aiken, L. S. (1995). The effect of different forms of centering in hierarchical linear models. Multivariate Behavior Research, 30, 1–21.
- Krondl, M., & Lau, D. (1982). Social determinants in human food selection. In L. M. Barker (Ed.), The psychobiology of human food selection (pp. 139–151). Westport, CT: AVI.
- Lavine, H., Thomsen, C. J., Zanna, M. P., & Borgida, E. (1998). On the primacy of affect in the determination of attitudes and behavior: The moderating role of affective-cognitive ambivalence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 398–421.
- Letarte, A., Dubé, L., & Troche, V. (1997). Similarities and differences in affective and cognitive origins of food likings and dislikings. Appetite, 28, 115–129.
- Lozano, D. I., Crites, S. L., & Aikman, S. N. (1999). Changes in food attitudes as a function of hunger. Appetite, 32, 207–218.
- Merrill, E. P., Kramer, F. M., Cardello, A., & Schutz, H. (2002). A comparison of satiety measures. Appetite, 39, 181–183.
- Millar, M. G., & Tesser, A. (1986). Effects of affective and cognitive focus on the attitude-behavior relation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 270–276.
- Millar, M. G., & Tesser, A. (1989). The effects of affective-cognitive consistency and thought on the attitude-behavior relation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 189–202.
- Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.
- Pliner, P., & Chaiken, S. (1990). Eating, social motives, and self-presentation in women and men. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 240–254.
- Povey, R., Conner, M., Sparks, P., James, R., & Shepherd, R. (2000). The theory of planned behaviour and healthy eating: Examining additive and moderating effects of social influence variables. Psychology & Health, 14, 991–1006.
- Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 431–449.
- Rappoport, L., Peters, G. R., Downey, R., McCann, T., & Huff-Corzine, L. (1993). Gender and age differences in food cognition. Appetite, 20, 33–52.
- Rasbash, J., Browne, W., Healy, M., Cameron, B., & Charlton, C. (2001). MLwiN (Version 1.10) [Computer Software]. London: Multilevel Models Project.
- Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
- Roininen, K., Lähteenmäki, L., & Tuorila, H. (1999). Quantification of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Appetite, 33, 71–88.
- Rozin, P. (1988). Social learning about food by humans. In T. R. Zentall, & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: A comparative approach (pp. 165–187). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1980). Psychological categorization of foods and non-foods: A preliminary taxonomy of food rejections. Appetite, 1, 193–201.
- Rozin, P., & Vollmecke, T. A. (1986). Food likes and dislikes. Annual Review of Nutrition, 6, 433–456.
- Schwarz, N. (2000). Social judgment and attitudes: Warmer, more social, and less conscious. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 149–176.
- Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
- Shavitt, S. (1989). Operationalizing functional theories of attitude. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure and function (pp. 311–337). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Shavitt, S., & Fazio, R. H. (1991). Effects of attribute salience on the consistency between attitudes and behavior predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 507–516.
- Shepherd, R. (1989). Factors influencing food preferences and choice. In R. Shepherd (Ed.), Handbook of the psychophysiology of human eating (pp. 3–24). New York: Wiley.
- Shepherd, R., & Farleigh, C. A. (1989). Sensory assessment of foods and the role of sensory attributes in determining food choice. In R. Shepherd (Ed.), Handbook of the psychophysiology of human eating (pp. 25–56). New York: Wiley.
- Smith, E. R. (1998). Mental representation and memory. In D. T. Gilbert, & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), The Handbook of social psychology ( 4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 391–444). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
- Sparks, P., Guthrie, C. A., & Shepherd, R. (1997). The dimensional structure of the perceived behavioral control construct. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 418–438.
- Sparks, P., Hedderly, D., & Shepherd, R. (1992). An investigation into the relationship between perceived control, attitude variability and the consumption of two common foods. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 55–71.
- Steptoe, A., Pollard, T. M., & Wardle, J. (1995). Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: The Food Choice Questionnaire. Appetite, 25, 267–284.
- Stricker, E. M., & Verbalis, J. G. (1990). Sodium appetite. In W. Willett (Ed.), Nutritional epidemiology (pp. 387–419). New York: Oxford.
- Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 71–83.
- Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of Sociology, 33, 529–554.
- Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying context effects in attitude measurement. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 299–314.
- Tuorila, H. (1991). Attitudes as determinants of food consumption. In Encyclopedia of Human Biology (Vol. 1, pp. 497–505). San Diego: Academic Press.
- Tuorila, H., Cardello, A. V., & Lesher, L. L. (1994). Antecedents and consequences of expectations related to fat-free and regular-fat foods. Appetite, 23, 247–263.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2001). The Surgeon General's call to action to prevent and decrease overweight and obesity. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
- Wainer, H. (2000). The centercept: An estimable and meaningful regression parameter. Psychological Science, 11(5), 434–436.
- Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don't mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753–763.
- Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
- Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In L. L. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 37–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Wilson, T. D., & Kraft, D. (1993). Why do I love thee?: Effects of repeated introspections about a dating relationship on attitudes toward the relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 409–418.
- Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection, attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: The disruptive effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 287–343). New York: Academic Press.
- Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., & LaFleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331–339.
- Wilson, T. D., Hodges, S. D., & LaFleur, S. J. (1995). Effects of introspecting about reasons: Inferring attitudes from accessible thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 16–28.
- Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Biek, M. (1995). Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information processing analysis. In R. E. Petty, & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 283–314). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Zanna, M. P., & Rempel, J. K. (1988). Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar-Tal, & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315–334). New York: Cambridge University Press.