The Roots of Moral Reasoning and Behavior in Infants
J. Kiley Hamlin
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorConor M. Steckler
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorJ. Kiley Hamlin
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorConor M. Steckler
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
Recent findings suggest that toddlers and infants engage in prosocial behaviors and evaluate potential social partners based on their morally relevant social acts. Together, this evidence suggests that some foundational aspects of human morality may stem from universal and unlearned features of the human mind. That is, despite the clear role of learning processes in much of moral development, basic motivations to cooperate with and help others, as well as an ability to judge third parties based on their prosocial and antisocial acts, may underlie and constrain those processes. Here, we review the current state of the literature on these topics and point to important remaining issues and future directions.
References
- Aknin, L. B., Hamlin, J. K., & Dunn, E. W. (2012). Giving leads to happiness in young children. Plos One, 7(6), 1–4. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039211
- Anderson, S., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(11), 1032–1037.
- Bloom, P. (2010). The moral life of babies. New York Times Magazine, 9 May.
- Dahl, A., Schuck, R. K., & Campos, J. J. (2013). Do young toddlers act on their social preferences? Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1964–1970. doi:10.1037/a0031460
- Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2010). Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy. Psychological Science, 21, 523–527.
- Fawcett, C., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Infants anticipate others' social preferences. Infant and Child Development, 21, 239–249. doi:10.1002/icd.739
- Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: infants' reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1012–1020. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x
- Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.
- Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants' social evaluations. Cognition, 128(3), 451–474. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.004
- Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: the role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 614.
- Hamlin, J. K. (under review). The eyes have it: The role of cues to agency and goal-directedness in infants' social evaluations. University of British Columbia.
- Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24(4), 589–594. doi:10.1177/0956797612457785
- Hamlin, J. K., Ullman, T., Tenenbaum, J., Goodman, N., & Baker, C. (2013). The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. Developmental Science, 16, 209–226.
- Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26, 30–39.
- Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2012). Who knows what's good to eat? Infants fail to match the food preferences of antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 27, 227–239.
- Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.
- Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13, 923–929.
- Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. doi:10.1073/pnas.1110306108
- Hay, D., Castle, J., Davies, L., Demetriou, H., & Stimson, C. (1999). Prosocial action in very early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(6), 905–916.
- Kenward, B., & Dahl, M. (2011). Preschoolers distribute scarce resources according to the moral valence of recipients' previous actions. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1054–1064.
- Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2014). Handbook of moral development. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). One-year-olds warn others about negative action outcomes. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 424–436.
- Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stages in the development of moral thought and action. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
- Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14, 402–408.
- Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., & Woodward, A. L. (2013). Friends or foes: Infants use shared evaluations to infer others' social relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 966–971.
- Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-Month-Olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 173–187.
- Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable dominance relations by human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 6862–6867.
- Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2013). Do infants detect indirect reciprocity? Cognition, 129(1), 102–113. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.006
-
Nesse, R. (2007). Runaway social selection for displays of partner value and altruism. Biological Theory, 2(2), 143–155.
10.1162/biot.2007.2.2.143 Google Scholar
- Rhee, S., Friedman, N. P., Boeldt, D. L., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., Knafo, A., …, Zahn-Waxler, C. (2013). Early concern and disregard for others as predictors of antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54(2), 157–166. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02574.x
- Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior & Development, 34(3), 447–458. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007
- Rottman, J., & Kelemen, D. (2012). Aliens behaving badly: Children's acquisition of novel purity-based morals. Cognition, 124(3), 356–360.
- Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175.
- Scarf, D., Imuta, K., Colombo, M., Hayne, H., & Botbol, M. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. Plos One, 7(8), 1–4. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042698
- Schmidt, M. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. Plos One, 6(10), 1–7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023223
- Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science (Sage Publications Inc.), 23(2), 196–204.
- Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. H., Yun, J., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18(1), 40–66. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00129.x
- Taber-Thomas, B., Asp, E., Koenigs, M., Sutterer, M., Anderson, S., & Tranel, D. (2014). Arrested development: Early prefrontal lesions impair the maturation of moral judgement. Brain, 137, 1254–1261. doi:10.1093/brain/awt377
- Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., & Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance. Science, 331, 477–480.
- Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
- Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 534–543.
- Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301–1303.
- Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1785–1788. doi:10.1037/a0013860
- Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013a). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18, 345–368.
- Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013b). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 338–350. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002
- Wellman, H. M., Lopez-Duran, S., LaBounty, J., & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 44, 618–623.
- Yamaguchi, M., Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & vanMarle, K. (2009). Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Developmental Science, 12(5), 746–752. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x
- Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126
Further Reading
- Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York, NY: Random House LLC.
- Hamlin, K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 186–193.
Citing Literature
Browse other articles of this reference work: