The Intrinsic Dynamics of Development
Paul van Geert
University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorMarijn van Dijk
University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorPaul van Geert
University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorMarijn van Dijk
University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
In this essay, we discuss an emergent developmental science. It provides an approach to development that redefines its theoretical and methodological basis in the general theory of complex dynamic systems. Its methodological research choices are in line with a focus on actual developmental processes, as they occur in individual cases, such as individual children, families, or relationships. Intraindividual variability based on frequent short-term as well as long-term measurements provides an important source of information. Theoretically, we advocate a model of a network of dynamically interacting components, generating a wide variety of developmental trajectories, in line with the idiosyncratic nature of developmental systems.
References
-
Bak, P. (1996). How Nature Works: The Science of Self-Organized Criticality. New York, NY: Springer.
10.1007/978-1-4757-5426-1 Google Scholar
- Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198.
- Cronbach, L. J. (1960). The essentials of psychological testing. New York, NY: Harper & Brothers.
- Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56, 391–430.
- De Ruiter-Wilcox, N., den Hartigh, J. R., Cox, R. F. A., Van Geert, P. L. C & Kunnen, E. S. (submitted). The temporal structure of state self-esteem variability during parent-child interactions: More than random fluctuations.
- Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Theoretical models of human development: Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, 6th ed., pp. 313–399). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
- Fischer, K. W., & Van Geert, P. L. C. (2014). Dynamic development of brain and behavior. In P. C. M. Molenaar, R. M. Lerner & K. M. Newell (Eds.), Handbook of developmental systems theory & methodology (pp. 287–315). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
- Flynn, E., & Siegler, R. (2007). Measuring change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research. Infant and Child Development, 16, 135–149.
- Freedland, R. L., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1994). Developmental changes in interlimb coordination: Transition to hands-and-knees crawling. Psychological Science, 5, 26–32.
-
Granott, N., Fischer, K. W., & Parziale, J. (2002). Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development. In N. Granott & J. Parziale (Eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning (pp. 1–28). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511489709.001 Google Scholar
-
Hollenstein, T. (2013). State space grids: Depicting dynamics across development. New York, NY: Springer.
10.1007/978-1-4614-5007-8 Google Scholar
- Hollenstein, T., Granic, I., Stoolmiller, M., & Snyder, J. (2004). Rigidity in parent-child interactions and the development of externalizing and internalizing behavior in early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 595–607.
-
Kloos, H., & Van Orden, G. C. (2009). Soft-assembled mechanisms for the grand theory. In J. P. Spencer, M. Thomas & J. McClelland (Eds.), Toward a new grand theory of development? Connectionism and dynamics systems theory reconsidered (pp. 253–267). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195300598.003.0012 Google Scholar
- Lavelli, M., Pantoja, A. P. F., Hsu, H., Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2006). Using microgenetic designs to study change processes. In D. M. Teti (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in developmental science (pp. 40–65). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
- Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Development, 71, 36–43.
- Lewis, M. D., Lamey, A. V., & Douglas, L. (1999). A new dynamic systems method for the analysis of early socioemotional development. Developmental Science, 2, 458–476.
- Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., Kunnen, S. E., & van Geert, P. L. (2009). Here we go again: A dynamic systems perspective on emotional rigidity across parent–adolescent conflicts. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 1364.
- Lord, F. M., & Novick, R. (1968). Statistical theories of mental testscores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
- Mainhard, M. T., Pennings, H. J., Wubbels, T., & Brekelmans, M. (2012). Mapping control and affiliation in teacher–student interaction with State Space Grids. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28(7), 1027–1037.
- Marchman, V. A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21, 339–339.
- Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., & Hollenstein, T. (2005). Social dynamics in the preschool. Developmental Review, 25, 299–327.
- Marwan, N. (2008). A historical review of recurrence plots. European Physical Journal, 164, 3–12.
- Marwan, N., Romano, M. C., Thiel, M., & Kurths, J. (2007). Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports, 438(5–6), 237–329.
-
McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., & Hogarty, P. S. (1977). Transitions in early mental development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 42, 108. doi:10.2307/1165992
10.2307/1165992 Google Scholar
- Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 112–117.
- Nunnally, J. C. (1970). Introduction to psychological measurement. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
- Schumacher, N., Ballato, L., & Van Geert, P. (2014). Using an agent-based model to simulate the development of risk behaviors during adolescence. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 17(3), 1–29.
- Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language, 6th edition ( 464–510). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
- Simonton, D. K. (2001). Talent development as a multidimensional, multiplicative, and dynamic process. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 39–43.
- Skinner, B. F. (1937). Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 272–279.
- Steenbeek, H., Jansen, L., & van Geert, P. (2012). Scaffolding dynamics and the emergence of problematic learning trajectories. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 64–75.
- Steenbeek, H. W., van der Aalsvoort, D., & van Geert, P. (2014). Collaborative play in young children as a complex dynamic system; revealing gender-related differences. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 18(3), 251–276.
- Steenbeek, H., & Van Geert, P. (2007). A dynamic systems approach to dyadic interaction in children: Emotional expression, action, dyadic play, and sociometric status. Developmental Review, 27(1), 1–40.
- Steenbeek, H., & Van Geert, P. (2008). An empirical validation of a dynamic systems model of interaction: Do children of different sociometric statuses differ in their dyadic play? Developmental Science, 11(2), 253–281.
- Steenbeek, H., & van Geert, P. (2013). The emergence of learning-teaching trajectories in education: A complex dynamic systems approach. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 17(2), 233–267.
- Thelen, E. (1985). Developmental origins of motor coordination: Leg movements in human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 1–22.
-
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.
10.7551/mitpress/2524.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Van Der Steen, S., Steenbeek, H., Van Dijk, M., & Van Geert, P. (2014). A process approach to children's understanding of scientific concepts: A longitudinal case study (Vol. 30, pp. 84–91). Learning and Individual Differences.
- Van Dijk, M., Hunnius, S., & van Geert, P. (2012). The dynamics of feeding during the introduction to solid food. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 226–239. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.01.001
- Van Dijk, M., & Van Geert, P. (2007). Wobbles, humps and sudden jumps: A case study of continuity, discontinuity, and variability in early language development. Infant and Child Development, 16, 7–33.
- Van Dijk, M., & Van Geert, P. (2014). The nature and meaning of intra-individual variability in development in the early life span. In M. Diehl, K. Hooker & M. Sliwinski (Eds.), Handbook of intra-individual variability across the life span. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge (in press).
- Van Dijk, M., van Geert, P., Korecky-Kroell, K., Maillochon, I., Laaha, S., Dressler, W. U., & Bassano, D. (2013). Adaptation between child directed speech and early child language; a developmental perspective. Language Learning: Manuscript accepted for publication.
- van Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53.
- Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos. New York, NY: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
- Van Geert, P. L. C. (1998). A dynamic systems model of basic developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky and beyond. Psychological Review, 105(4), 634–677.
- Van Geert, P. (2011). The contribution of complex dynamic systems to development. Child Development Perspectives, 5(4), 273–278.
- Van Geert, P., den Hartigh, J. R., Steenbeek, H. W. & Van Dijk, M. (submitted). The development of expertise and exceptional performance: A dynamic network model.
- Van Geert, P., & Steenbeek, H. (2005a). The dynamics of scaffolding. New Ideas in Psychology, 23(3), 115–128.
- Van Geert, P., & Steenbeek, H. (2005b). Explaining after by before. Basic aspects of a dynamic systems approach to the study of development. Developmental Review, 25(3–4), 408–442.
- Van Geert, P., & Van Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability, new tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340–374.
- Webber, C. L., Jr. & Zbilut, J. P. (2005). Recurrence quantification analysis of nonlinear dynamical systems. In M. A. Riley & G. C. Van Orden (Eds.), Tutorials in contemporary nonlinear methods for the behavioral sciences (pp. 26–94). Washington, DC: National Science Foundation.
- Wijnants, M. L., Bosman, A. M. T., Hasselman, F., Cox, R. F. A., & Van Orden, G. C. (2009). 1/f scaling in movement time changes with practice in precision aiming. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 13, 79–98.
- Wijnants, M. L., Hasselman, F., Cox, R. F. A., Bosman, A. M. T., & Van Orden, G. C. (2012). An interaction-dominant perspective on reading fluency and dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 62, 100–119.
Citing Literature
Browse other articles of this reference work: